Peat: An Integrative Model of Attribution Processes

Publisher Summary This chapter discusses a core model of attribution processes, PEAT, stated in probabilities. Two mechanisms are primarily responsible for the ability of PEAT to integrate attribution theory. PEAT has been able to provide some improvement in the clarity of the concepts used in attribution theory because it has reduced those concepts to the precise vocabulary of probabilities. The most important clarification was the distinction between contrast-based and prediction-based definitions of covariation information. A second clarification was the distinction, expressed in probabilities, between causal and characteristic attributions. PEAT is also promising as a generator of empirical research. Some of that research may arise from its comments on other theories, for example, PEAT states that when observers are operating under correspondent inference theory (CIT), they will spontaneously make assumptions about consistency even though CIT has no theoretical role for consistency. The chapter concludes that further theoretical work needs to be done to extend the basics of PEAT described in new areas. PEAT promises to generate both theoretical and empirical work.

[1]  J. Crocker,et al.  Cognitive Processes in the Revision of Stereotypic Beliefs , 1983 .

[2]  Baruch Fischhoff,et al.  Focusing Techniques: A Shortcut to Improving Probability Judgments? , 1984 .

[3]  Ira Solomon,et al.  Contextual effects on the calibration of probabilistic judgments. , 1985 .

[4]  A. Tversky,et al.  On the psychology of prediction , 1973 .

[5]  L. A. McArthur The how and what of why: Some determinants and consequences of causal attribution. , 1972 .

[6]  J. Howard,et al.  Further Appraisal of Correspondent Inference Theory , 1985 .

[7]  A. Kruglanski Lay epistemo-logic—process and contents: Another look at attribution theory. , 1980 .

[8]  R. Scott Tindale,et al.  The influence of the accuracy of individuating information on the use of base rate information in probability judgment , 1988 .

[9]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  Lay persons' sensitivity to statistical information: The case of high perceived applicability. , 1984 .

[10]  H. Kelley The processes of causal attribution. , 1973 .

[11]  I. Ajzen,et al.  A Bayesian analysis of attribution processes. , 1975 .

[12]  V. A. Harris,et al.  The Attribution of Attitudes , 1967 .

[13]  J Crocker,et al.  Another look at sex stereotypes and social judgments: an analysis of the social perceiver's use of subjective probabilities. , 1985, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[14]  H. Kelley,et al.  Attribution theory and research. , 1980, Annual review of psychology.

[15]  E. E. Jones,et al.  From Acts To Dispositions The Attribution Process In Person Perception1 , 1965 .

[16]  R. Hogarth,et al.  Ambiguity and Uncertainty in Probabilistic Inference. , 1985 .

[17]  Denis J. Hilton,et al.  The explanation of occurrences and non-occurrences: A test of the inductive logic model of causal attribution , 1987 .

[18]  Miles Hewstone,et al.  COVARIATION AND CAUSAL ATTRIBUTION - A LOGICAL MODEL OF THE INTUITIVE ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE , 1987 .

[19]  R. Hogarth,et al.  Judging probable cause. , 1986 .

[20]  D. Ronis,et al.  Discounting and Augmentation , 1982 .

[21]  D. Hilton,et al.  Knowledge-Based Causal Attribution: The Abnormal Conditions Focus Model , 1986 .

[22]  M. Brewer,et al.  A schematic model of dispositional attribution in interpersonal perception. , 1979 .

[23]  D. Budescu,et al.  Consistency in interpretation of probabilistic phrases , 1985 .

[24]  R. D. Hansen,et al.  Discounting and augmenting facilitative and inhibitory forces: The winner takes almost all. , 1985 .

[25]  A. Tversky,et al.  Causal Schemata in Judgments under Uncertainty , 1982 .

[26]  R. A. Wicklund,et al.  When unexpected behavior is attributed to the environment , 1976 .

[27]  Denis J. Hilton,et al.  Contemporary science and natural explanation : commonsense conceptions of causality , 1988 .

[28]  M. Hewstone,et al.  The social dimension: Social dimensions of attribution , 1984 .

[29]  B. Fischhoff,et al.  Behavioral Decision Theory , 1977 .

[30]  E. E. Jones,et al.  Role playing variations and their informational value for person perception. , 1961, Journal of abnormal and social psychology.

[31]  Frank D. Fincham,et al.  Attribution Theory and Research: Conceptual, Developmental and Social Dimensions , 1983 .

[32]  Sandra J. Milberg,et al.  Category-based and attribute-based reactions to others: Some informational conditions of stereotyping and individuating processes , 1987 .

[33]  D. Hilton,et al.  The knowledge-structure and inductivist strategies in causal attribution: A direct comparison , 1988 .

[34]  S. Worchel,et al.  Prior expectancy and behavioral extremity as determinants of attitude attribution , 1971 .

[35]  Saul M. Kassin,et al.  Consensus information, prediction, and causal attribution: A review of the literature and issues. , 1979 .

[36]  Miles Hewstone,et al.  A re-examination of the roles of consensus, consistency and distinctiveness: Kelley's cube revisited† , 1983 .

[37]  E. Sadalla,et al.  The Search for Predictable Settings. Situational Prototypes, Constraint, and Behavioral Variation , 1985 .

[38]  J. Harvey,et al.  Current issues in attribution theory and research. , 1984, Annual review of psychology.

[39]  A recency effect in attitude attribution1 , 1976 .

[40]  Willem A. Wagenaar,et al.  Calibration of probability assessments by professional blackjack dealers, statistical experts, and lay people , 1985 .