SOLAR versus SERIOL revisited

This paper compares two approaches to modelling orthographic processing, the Self-Organising Lexical Acquisition and Recognition (SOLAR; Davis, 1999, in press) and the Sequential Encoding Regulated by Inputs to Oscillating Letter units (SERIOL; Whitney, 2001, 2004) models, following up on a previous analysis by Whitney (2008). I provide a brief overview of the SOLAR model, and its key similarities to and differences from the SERIOL model, focusing in particular on the different mechanisms underlying the formation of the positional gradient in the two models. I also discuss the neural implementation of the SOLAR model's lexical matching algorithm, and its plausibility. In the final part of the paper I discuss empirical attempts to adjudicate between the two models, focusing on the masked form priming paradigm, as well as the use of theoretical match values to test model predictions. It is concluded that the SOLAR model provides an account of visual word identification that is neurally plausible and that succeeds in explaining critical orthographical similarity data, but that the SERIOL model does not satisfy these constraints.

[1]  R. H. Arnott,et al.  Interaural Time Difference Discrimination Thresholds for Single Neurons in the Inferior Colliculus of Guinea Pigs , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  L A JEFFRESS,et al.  A place theory of sound localization. , 1948, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[3]  M. R. Mehta,et al.  Role of experience and oscillations in transforming a rate code into a temporal code , 2002, Nature.

[4]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  Representation and processing of derived words , 1987 .

[5]  N Suga,et al.  Combination-sensitive neurons in the medial geniculate body of the mustached bat: encoding of relative velocity information. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  James T. Townsend,et al.  Lateral masking for letters with unlimited viewing time , 1971 .

[7]  Danko Nikolic,et al.  Spatiotemporal Structure in Large Neuronal Networks Detected from Cross-Correlation , 2006, Neural Computation.

[8]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  How does interhemispheric communication in visual word recognition work? Deciding between early and late integration accounts of the split fovea theory , 2009, Brain and Language.

[9]  Manuel Perea,et al.  Transposed-letter effects: Consonants, vowels and letter frequency , 2008 .

[10]  J. Bowers,et al.  Automatic semantic activation of embedded words: Is there a hat in that? , 2005 .

[11]  K. Forster,et al.  Masked priming with graphemically related forms: Repetition or partial activation? , 1987 .

[12]  Barbara J. Juhasz,et al.  Eye movements and the use of parafoveal word length information in reading. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  M. Coltheart,et al.  Treatment of irregular word spelling in developmental surface dysgraphia , 2005, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[14]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Letter position information and printed word perception: the relative-position priming constraint. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  T. M. Ellison,et al.  Eye-fixation behavior, lexical storage, and visual word recognition in a split processing model. , 2000, Psychological review.

[16]  C. Davis,et al.  More words in the neighborhood: Interference in lexical decision due to deletion neighbors , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[17]  J. Grainger,et al.  Masked morphological priming in visual word recognition. , 1991 .

[18]  Carol Whitney,et al.  Investigations into the Neural Basis of Structured Representations , 2004 .

[19]  M. Carreiras,et al.  Do Transposed-Letter Similarity Effects Occur at a Prelexical Phonological Level? , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[20]  K. Forster,et al.  Criterion bias and search sequence bias in word recognition , 1981, Memory & cognition.

[21]  A. Angrilli,et al.  Inverted EEG theta lateralization in dyslexic children during phonological processing , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Theta band power changes in normal and dyslexic children , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[23]  I. Fraga,et al.  Transposed-letter and laterality effects in lexical decision , 2006, Brain and Language.

[24]  M. Konishi,et al.  Axonal delay lines for time measurement in the owl's brainstem. , 1988, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[25]  Jason R. Perry,et al.  An evaluation of the interactive-activation model using masked partial-word priming , 2008 .

[26]  S. Lupker,et al.  Masked inhibitory priming in english: evidence for lexical inhibition. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  Manuel Perea,et al.  Transposed-letter effects in reading: evidence from eye movements and parafoveal preview. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[28]  Philip T Quinlan,et al.  Orthographic processing in visual word identification , 1990, Cognitive Psychology.

[29]  Manuel Perea,et al.  Re(de)fining the orthographic neighborhood: the role of addition and deletion neighbors in lexical decision and reading. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  C. Whitney How the brain encodes the order of letters in a printed word: The SERIOL model and selective literature review , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[31]  M Mason,et al.  Recognition time for letters and nonletters: effects of serial position, array size, and processing order. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[32]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  Theoretical analysis of interhemispheric transfer costs in visual word recognition , 2008 .

[33]  J Grainger,et al.  The role of letter identity and letter position in orthographic priming , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[34]  Jeffrey S. Bowers,et al.  Interfering neighbours: The impact of novel word learning on the identification of visually similar words , 2005, Cognition.

[35]  E. Averbach,et al.  Short-term memory in vision , 1961 .

[36]  J. Grainger,et al.  Priming word recognition with orthographic neighbors: effects of relative prime-target frequency. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[37]  W Reichardt,et al.  Autocorrelation, a principle for evaluation of sensory information by the central nervous system , 1961 .

[38]  Nicola J. Pitchford,et al.  Effect of orthographic transparency on letter position encoding: A comparison of Greek and English monoscriptal and biscriptal readers , 2008 .

[39]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  A study of relative-position priming with superset primes. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[40]  Jeffrey S Bowers,et al.  What do letter migration errors reveal about letter position coding in visual word recognition? , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[41]  M. Nicholls,et al.  Sequential processing in hemispheric word recognition: The impact of initial letter discriminability on the OUP naming effect , 2005, Brain and Language.

[42]  J. van Santen,et al.  Elaborated Reichardt detectors. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[43]  Stephen Grossberg,et al.  Working Memory Networks for Learning Temporal Order with Application to Three-Dimensional Visual Object Recognition , 1992, Neural Computation.

[44]  J. Grainger,et al.  Letter position coding in printed word perception: Effects of repeated and transposed letters , 2004 .

[45]  H. BOUMA,et al.  Interaction Effects in Parafoveal Letter Recognition , 1970, Nature.

[46]  M. Sigman,et al.  The neural code for written words: a proposal , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[47]  Stephen Grossberg,et al.  A Theory of Human Memory: Self-Organization and Performance of Sensory-Motor Codes, Maps, and Plans , 1982 .

[48]  Matthew H. Davis,et al.  Is there a ‘fete’ in ‘fetish’? Effects of orthographic opacity on morpho-orthographic segmentation in visual word recognition , 2008 .

[49]  Sarah J. White,et al.  Raeding Wrods With Jubmled Lettres , 2006, Psychological science.

[50]  G. Buzsáki,et al.  Spike train dynamics predicts theta-related phase precession in hippocampal pyramidal cells , 2002, Nature.

[51]  J. Licklider,et al.  A duplex theory of pitch perception , 1951, Experientia.

[52]  Stephen Grossberg,et al.  Working memory networks for learning multiple groupings of temporally ordered events: applications to 3-D visual object recognition , 1991, IJCNN-91-Seattle International Joint Conference on Neural Networks.

[53]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Serial position effects in the identification of letters, digits, and symbols. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[54]  Clive Frankish,et al.  Short Article: Lexical and Sublexical Processes in the Perception of Transposed-Letter Anagrams , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[55]  Johannes C. Ziegler,et al.  A developmental perspective on the neural code for written words , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[56]  D. Norris,et al.  The primacy model: a new model of immediate serial recall. , 1998, Psychological review.

[57]  D. Green,et al.  Detecting targets in letter and non-letter arrays. , 1982, Canadian journal of psychology.

[58]  S. Lupker,et al.  Does jugde activate COURT? Transposed-letter similarity effects in masked associative priming , 2003, Memory & cognition.

[59]  K. Rastle,et al.  The processing of singular and plural nouns in French and English , 2004 .

[60]  Dennis Norris,et al.  Perception as evidence accumulation and Bayesian inference: insights from masked priming. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[61]  Colin J. Davis,et al.  The self-organising lexical acquisition and recognition (SOLAR) model of visual word recognition. , 2001 .

[62]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Cracking the orthographic code: An introduction , 2008 .

[63]  D. Levi Crowding—An essential bottleneck for object recognition: A mini-review , 2008, Vision Research.

[64]  D. Georgescauld Local Cortical Circuits, An Electrophysiological Study , 1983 .

[65]  Matthew H. Davis,et al.  Morphological decomposition based on the analysis of orthography , 2008 .

[66]  K. Forster,et al.  Lexical storage and retrieval of prefixed words , 1975 .

[67]  Jeffrey S Bowers,et al.  Contrasting five different theories of letter position coding: evidence from orthographic similarity effects. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[68]  P. Joris A dogged pursuit of coincidence. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[69]  Professor Moshe Abeles,et al.  Local Cortical Circuits , 1982, Studies of Brain Function.

[70]  Dennis Norris,et al.  Transposed-letter priming of prelexical orthographic representations. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[71]  B E Butler,et al.  On the nature of perceptual limits in vision , 1986, Psychological research.

[72]  M. Taft,et al.  Lack of Phonological Mediation in a Semantic Categorization Task , 1998 .

[73]  M. Brysbaert,et al.  Neighborhood-frequency effects when primes and targets are of different lengths , 2000, Psychological research.

[74]  Jamie I. D. Campbell,et al.  Identification, localization, and “iconic memory”: An evaluation of the bar-probe task , 1981, Memory & cognition.

[75]  S. Andrews,et al.  Eye movements and morphological segmentation of compound words: There is a mouse in mousetrap , 2004 .

[76]  W. Reichardt,et al.  Autocorrelation, a principle for the evaluation of sensory information by the central nervous system , 1961 .

[77]  S. Lupker,et al.  Can CANISO activate CASINO? Transposed-letter similarity effects with nonadjacent letter positions ☆ , 2004 .

[78]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Graded effects of number of inserted letters in superset priming. , 2008, Experimental psychology.

[79]  W. Estes,et al.  Serial position functions for letter identification at brief and extended exposure durations , 1976 .

[80]  Carol Whitney,et al.  Comparison of the SERIOL and SOLAR theories of letter-position encoding , 2008, Brain and Language.

[81]  S. Lupker,et al.  Sandwich priming: a method for overcoming the limitations of masked priming by reducing lexical competitor effects. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[82]  Danko Nikolic,et al.  Temporal dynamics of information content carried by neurons in the primary visual cortex , 2006, NIPS.

[83]  Manuel Perea,et al.  Transposed-Letter Confusability Effects in Masked Form Priming , 2003 .

[84]  D J Mewhort,et al.  Processing spatial information and the selective-masking effect , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.

[85]  S. M. Chambers Letter and order information in lexical access , 1979 .

[86]  S. Andrews Lexical Retrieval and Selection Processes: Effects of Transposed-Letter Confusability , 1996 .

[87]  A Borst,et al.  Fly motion vision is based on Reichardt detectors regardless of the signal-to-noise ratio. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[88]  T. Yin,et al.  Interaural time sensitivity in medial superior olive of cat. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[89]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Modeling letter position coding in printed word perception , 2004 .

[90]  S. Grossberg,et al.  Metabotropic Glutamate Receptor Activation in Cerebellar Purkinje Cells as Substrate for Adaptive Timing of the Classically Conditioned Eye-Blink Response , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[91]  J. Masterson,et al.  Effect of orthographic processes on letter position encoding , 2008 .

[92]  Colin J. Davis,et al.  Factors underlying masked priming effects in competitive network models of visual word recognition , 2003 .

[93]  Kenneth I. Forster,et al.  Masked form priming with extreme transposition , 2008 .

[94]  J. Hoffmann,et al.  The European Society for Cognitive Psychology , 1999 .

[95]  Bruno Rossion,et al.  The Speed of Orthographic Processing during Lexical Decision: Electrophysiological Evidence for Independent Coding of Letter Identity and Letter Position in Visual Word Recognition , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[96]  George Houghton,et al.  The problem of serial order: a neural network model of sequence learning and recall , 1990 .

[97]  S. Grossberg Some Networks That Can Learn, Remember, and Reproduce any Number of Complicated Space-Time Patterns, I , 1969 .

[98]  S. Lewandowsky The Rewards and Hazards of Computer Simulations , 1993 .

[99]  James L. McClelland,et al.  An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. , 1981 .

[100]  Colin J Davis,et al.  The spatial coding model of visual word identification. , 2010, Psychological review.

[101]  V. Braitenberg Is the cerebellar cortex a biological clock in the millisecond range? , 1967, Progress in brain research.

[102]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Letter visibility and the viewing position effect in visual word recognition , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.