SOLAR versus SERIOL revisited
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] R. H. Arnott,et al. Interaural Time Difference Discrimination Thresholds for Single Neurons in the Inferior Colliculus of Guinea Pigs , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[2] L A JEFFRESS,et al. A place theory of sound localization. , 1948, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[3] M. R. Mehta,et al. Role of experience and oscillations in transforming a rate code into a temporal code , 2002, Nature.
[4] Alfonso Caramazza,et al. Representation and processing of derived words , 1987 .
[5] N Suga,et al. Combination-sensitive neurons in the medial geniculate body of the mustached bat: encoding of relative velocity information. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.
[6] James T. Townsend,et al. Lateral masking for letters with unlimited viewing time , 1971 .
[7] Danko Nikolic,et al. Spatiotemporal Structure in Large Neuronal Networks Detected from Cross-Correlation , 2006, Neural Computation.
[8] Marc Brysbaert,et al. How does interhemispheric communication in visual word recognition work? Deciding between early and late integration accounts of the split fovea theory , 2009, Brain and Language.
[9] Manuel Perea,et al. Transposed-letter effects: Consonants, vowels and letter frequency , 2008 .
[10] J. Bowers,et al. Automatic semantic activation of embedded words: Is there a hat in that? , 2005 .
[11] K. Forster,et al. Masked priming with graphemically related forms: Repetition or partial activation? , 1987 .
[12] Barbara J. Juhasz,et al. Eye movements and the use of parafoveal word length information in reading. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[13] M. Coltheart,et al. Treatment of irregular word spelling in developmental surface dysgraphia , 2005, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[14] Jonathan Grainger,et al. Letter position information and printed word perception: the relative-position priming constraint. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[15] T. M. Ellison,et al. Eye-fixation behavior, lexical storage, and visual word recognition in a split processing model. , 2000, Psychological review.
[16] C. Davis,et al. More words in the neighborhood: Interference in lexical decision due to deletion neighbors , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[17] J. Grainger,et al. Masked morphological priming in visual word recognition. , 1991 .
[18] Carol Whitney,et al. Investigations into the Neural Basis of Structured Representations , 2004 .
[19] M. Carreiras,et al. Do Transposed-Letter Similarity Effects Occur at a Prelexical Phonological Level? , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[20] K. Forster,et al. Criterion bias and search sequence bias in word recognition , 1981, Memory & cognition.
[21] A. Angrilli,et al. Inverted EEG theta lateralization in dyslexic children during phonological processing , 2006, Neuropsychologia.
[22] H. Wimmer,et al. Theta band power changes in normal and dyslexic children , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[23] I. Fraga,et al. Transposed-letter and laterality effects in lexical decision , 2006, Brain and Language.
[24] M. Konishi,et al. Axonal delay lines for time measurement in the owl's brainstem. , 1988, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[25] Jason R. Perry,et al. An evaluation of the interactive-activation model using masked partial-word priming , 2008 .
[26] S. Lupker,et al. Masked inhibitory priming in english: evidence for lexical inhibition. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[27] Manuel Perea,et al. Transposed-letter effects in reading: evidence from eye movements and parafoveal preview. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[28] Philip T Quinlan,et al. Orthographic processing in visual word identification , 1990, Cognitive Psychology.
[29] Manuel Perea,et al. Re(de)fining the orthographic neighborhood: the role of addition and deletion neighbors in lexical decision and reading. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[30] C. Whitney. How the brain encodes the order of letters in a printed word: The SERIOL model and selective literature review , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[31] M Mason,et al. Recognition time for letters and nonletters: effects of serial position, array size, and processing order. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[32] Marc Brysbaert,et al. Theoretical analysis of interhemispheric transfer costs in visual word recognition , 2008 .
[33] J Grainger,et al. The role of letter identity and letter position in orthographic priming , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.
[34] Jeffrey S. Bowers,et al. Interfering neighbours: The impact of novel word learning on the identification of visually similar words , 2005, Cognition.
[35] E. Averbach,et al. Short-term memory in vision , 1961 .
[36] J. Grainger,et al. Priming word recognition with orthographic neighbors: effects of relative prime-target frequency. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[37] W Reichardt,et al. Autocorrelation, a principle for evaluation of sensory information by the central nervous system , 1961 .
[38] Nicola J. Pitchford,et al. Effect of orthographic transparency on letter position encoding: A comparison of Greek and English monoscriptal and biscriptal readers , 2008 .
[39] Jonathan Grainger,et al. A study of relative-position priming with superset primes. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[40] Jeffrey S Bowers,et al. What do letter migration errors reveal about letter position coding in visual word recognition? , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[41] M. Nicholls,et al. Sequential processing in hemispheric word recognition: The impact of initial letter discriminability on the OUP naming effect , 2005, Brain and Language.
[42] J. van Santen,et al. Elaborated Reichardt detectors. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[43] Stephen Grossberg,et al. Working Memory Networks for Learning Temporal Order with Application to Three-Dimensional Visual Object Recognition , 1992, Neural Computation.
[44] J. Grainger,et al. Letter position coding in printed word perception: Effects of repeated and transposed letters , 2004 .
[45] H. BOUMA,et al. Interaction Effects in Parafoveal Letter Recognition , 1970, Nature.
[46] M. Sigman,et al. The neural code for written words: a proposal , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[47] Stephen Grossberg,et al. A Theory of Human Memory: Self-Organization and Performance of Sensory-Motor Codes, Maps, and Plans , 1982 .
[48] Matthew H. Davis,et al. Is there a ‘fete’ in ‘fetish’? Effects of orthographic opacity on morpho-orthographic segmentation in visual word recognition , 2008 .
[49] Sarah J. White,et al. Raeding Wrods With Jubmled Lettres , 2006, Psychological science.
[50] G. Buzsáki,et al. Spike train dynamics predicts theta-related phase precession in hippocampal pyramidal cells , 2002, Nature.
[51] J. Licklider,et al. A duplex theory of pitch perception , 1951, Experientia.
[52] Stephen Grossberg,et al. Working memory networks for learning multiple groupings of temporally ordered events: applications to 3-D visual object recognition , 1991, IJCNN-91-Seattle International Joint Conference on Neural Networks.
[53] Jonathan Grainger,et al. Serial position effects in the identification of letters, digits, and symbols. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[54] Clive Frankish,et al. Short Article: Lexical and Sublexical Processes in the Perception of Transposed-Letter Anagrams , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[55] Johannes C. Ziegler,et al. A developmental perspective on the neural code for written words , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[56] D. Norris,et al. The primacy model: a new model of immediate serial recall. , 1998, Psychological review.
[57] D. Green,et al. Detecting targets in letter and non-letter arrays. , 1982, Canadian journal of psychology.
[58] S. Lupker,et al. Does jugde activate COURT? Transposed-letter similarity effects in masked associative priming , 2003, Memory & cognition.
[59] K. Rastle,et al. The processing of singular and plural nouns in French and English , 2004 .
[60] Dennis Norris,et al. Perception as evidence accumulation and Bayesian inference: insights from masked priming. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[61] Colin J. Davis,et al. The self-organising lexical acquisition and recognition (SOLAR) model of visual word recognition. , 2001 .
[62] Jonathan Grainger,et al. Cracking the orthographic code: An introduction , 2008 .
[63] D. Levi. Crowding—An essential bottleneck for object recognition: A mini-review , 2008, Vision Research.
[64] D. Georgescauld. Local Cortical Circuits, An Electrophysiological Study , 1983 .
[65] Matthew H. Davis,et al. Morphological decomposition based on the analysis of orthography , 2008 .
[66] K. Forster,et al. Lexical storage and retrieval of prefixed words , 1975 .
[67] Jeffrey S Bowers,et al. Contrasting five different theories of letter position coding: evidence from orthographic similarity effects. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[68] P. Joris. A dogged pursuit of coincidence. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.
[69] Professor Moshe Abeles,et al. Local Cortical Circuits , 1982, Studies of Brain Function.
[70] Dennis Norris,et al. Transposed-letter priming of prelexical orthographic representations. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[71] B E Butler,et al. On the nature of perceptual limits in vision , 1986, Psychological research.
[72] M. Taft,et al. Lack of Phonological Mediation in a Semantic Categorization Task , 1998 .
[73] M. Brysbaert,et al. Neighborhood-frequency effects when primes and targets are of different lengths , 2000, Psychological research.
[74] Jamie I. D. Campbell,et al. Identification, localization, and “iconic memory”: An evaluation of the bar-probe task , 1981, Memory & cognition.
[75] S. Andrews,et al. Eye movements and morphological segmentation of compound words: There is a mouse in mousetrap , 2004 .
[76] W. Reichardt,et al. Autocorrelation, a principle for the evaluation of sensory information by the central nervous system , 1961 .
[77] S. Lupker,et al. Can CANISO activate CASINO? Transposed-letter similarity effects with nonadjacent letter positions ☆ , 2004 .
[78] Jonathan Grainger,et al. Graded effects of number of inserted letters in superset priming. , 2008, Experimental psychology.
[79] W. Estes,et al. Serial position functions for letter identification at brief and extended exposure durations , 1976 .
[80] Carol Whitney,et al. Comparison of the SERIOL and SOLAR theories of letter-position encoding , 2008, Brain and Language.
[81] S. Lupker,et al. Sandwich priming: a method for overcoming the limitations of masked priming by reducing lexical competitor effects. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[82] Danko Nikolic,et al. Temporal dynamics of information content carried by neurons in the primary visual cortex , 2006, NIPS.
[83] Manuel Perea,et al. Transposed-Letter Confusability Effects in Masked Form Priming , 2003 .
[84] D J Mewhort,et al. Processing spatial information and the selective-masking effect , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.
[85] S. M. Chambers. Letter and order information in lexical access , 1979 .
[86] S. Andrews. Lexical Retrieval and Selection Processes: Effects of Transposed-Letter Confusability , 1996 .
[87] A Borst,et al. Fly motion vision is based on Reichardt detectors regardless of the signal-to-noise ratio. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[88] T. Yin,et al. Interaural time sensitivity in medial superior olive of cat. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.
[89] Jonathan Grainger,et al. Modeling letter position coding in printed word perception , 2004 .
[90] S. Grossberg,et al. Metabotropic Glutamate Receptor Activation in Cerebellar Purkinje Cells as Substrate for Adaptive Timing of the Classically Conditioned Eye-Blink Response , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[91] J. Masterson,et al. Effect of orthographic processes on letter position encoding , 2008 .
[92] Colin J. Davis,et al. Factors underlying masked priming effects in competitive network models of visual word recognition , 2003 .
[93] Kenneth I. Forster,et al. Masked form priming with extreme transposition , 2008 .
[94] J. Hoffmann,et al. The European Society for Cognitive Psychology , 1999 .
[95] Bruno Rossion,et al. The Speed of Orthographic Processing during Lexical Decision: Electrophysiological Evidence for Independent Coding of Letter Identity and Letter Position in Visual Word Recognition , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[96] George Houghton,et al. The problem of serial order: a neural network model of sequence learning and recall , 1990 .
[97] S. Grossberg. Some Networks That Can Learn, Remember, and Reproduce any Number of Complicated Space-Time Patterns, I , 1969 .
[98] S. Lewandowsky. The Rewards and Hazards of Computer Simulations , 1993 .
[99] James L. McClelland,et al. An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. , 1981 .
[100] Colin J Davis,et al. The spatial coding model of visual word identification. , 2010, Psychological review.
[101] V. Braitenberg. Is the cerebellar cortex a biological clock in the millisecond range? , 1967, Progress in brain research.
[102] Jonathan Grainger,et al. Letter visibility and the viewing position effect in visual word recognition , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.