The relationship between reinforcement and explicit control during visuomotor adaptation
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Maurice A. Smith,et al. The Decay of Motor Memories Is Independent of Context Change Detection , 2015, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[2] David M. Huberdeau,et al. Dual-process decomposition in human sensorimotor adaptation , 2015, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[3] M. Hallett,et al. Rapid plasticity of human cortical movement representation induced by practice. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.
[4] R. Sun,et al. The interaction of the explicit and the implicit in skill learning: a dual-process approach. , 2005, Psychological review.
[5] Welber Marinovic,et al. Estimating the implicit component of visuomotor rotation learning by constraining movement preparation time , 2017, bioRxiv.
[6] Ethan R. Buch,et al. Noninvasive cortical stimulation enhances motor skill acquisition over multiple days through an effect on consolidation , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[7] Sarah E. Pekny,et al. Protection and Expression of Human Motor Memories , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[8] P. Dayan,et al. Model-based influences on humans’ choices and striatal prediction errors , 2011, Neuron.
[9] J. Krakauer,et al. A computational neuroanatomy for motor control , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.
[10] P. Viviani,et al. Pointing errors reflect biases in the perception of the initial hand position. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.
[11] Joseph M. Galea,et al. Predicting explorative motor learning using decision-making and motor noise , 2017, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[12] Richard B. Ivry,et al. Characteristics of Implicit Sensorimotor Adaptation Revealed by Task-irrelevant Clamped Feedback , 2017, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[13] O. Donchin,et al. Awareness of Sensorimotor Adaptation to Visual Rotations of Different Size , 2015, PloS one.
[14] Nicolas Schweighofer,et al. Between-Trial Forgetting Due to Interference and Time in Motor Adaptation , 2015, PloS one.
[15] Jordan A Taylor,et al. Explicit and Implicit Processes Constitute the Fast and Slow Processes of Sensorimotor Learning , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[16] Jeremy C. Rietschel,et al. Increased reward in ankle robotics training enhances motor control and cortical efficiency in stroke. , 2014, Journal of rehabilitation research and development.
[17] D. Wolpert,et al. Effective reinforcement learning following cerebellar damage requires a balance between exploration and motor noise , 2015, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[18] Reza Shadmehr,et al. Learning from Sensory and Reward Prediction Errors during Motor Adaptation , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[19] P. Dayan,et al. Uncertainty-based competition between prefrontal and dorsolateral striatal systems for behavioral control , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[20] Krista M Bond,et al. Flexible explicit but rigid implicit learning in a visuomotor adaptation task. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.
[21] S. Wise,et al. Mechanisms of use-dependent plasticity in the human motor cortex. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[22] J. Galea,et al. The effects of reward and punishment on motor skill learning , 2018, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.
[23] Y. Kojima,et al. Selective reward affects the rate of saccade adaptation , 2017, Neuroscience.
[24] D H Brainard,et al. The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[25] J. Rothwell,et al. Reward and punishment enhance motor adaptation in stroke , 2017, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry.
[26] Hiroaki Gomi,et al. Multiple Motor Learning Strategies in Visuomotor Rotation , 2010, PloS one.
[27] John W Krakauer,et al. Motor learning and consolidation: the case of visuomotor rotation. , 2009, Advances in experimental medicine and biology.
[28] J. Krakauer,et al. An Implicit Plan Overrides an Explicit Strategy during Visuomotor Adaptation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[29] Stephan Riek,et al. Savings for visuomotor adaptation require prior history of error, not prior repetition of successful actions. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.
[30] Peter Dayan,et al. Bonsai Trees in Your Head: How the Pavlovian System Sculpts Goal-Directed Choices by Pruning Decision Trees , 2012, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[31] P. Dayan,et al. States versus Rewards: Dissociable Neural Prediction Error Signals Underlying Model-Based and Model-Free Reinforcement Learning , 2010, Neuron.
[32] J. Krakauer,et al. Explicit and Implicit Contributions to Learning in a Sensorimotor Adaptation Task , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[33] Michael J. Frank,et al. Faculty Opinions recommendation of States versus rewards: dissociable neural prediction error signals underlying model-based and model-free reinforcement learning. , 2010 .
[34] Vincent S. Huang,et al. Rethinking Motor Learning and Savings in Adaptation Paradigms: Model-Free Memory for Successful Actions Combines with Internal Models , 2011, Neuron.
[35] Dylan A. Simon,et al. Neural Correlates of Forward Planning in a Spatial Decision Task in Humans , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[36] R. Shadmehr,et al. Interacting Adaptive Processes with Different Timescales Underlie Short-Term Motor Learning , 2006, PLoS biology.
[37] Rachael D. Seidler,et al. Contributions of Spatial Working Memory to Visuomotor Learning , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[38] Richard S. Sutton,et al. Reinforcement Learning: An Introduction , 1998, IEEE Trans. Neural Networks.
[39] Philippe Lefèvre,et al. Formation of model-free motor memories during motor adaptation depends on perturbation schedule. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.
[40] D. Wolpert,et al. Internal models in the cerebellum , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[41] A. Haith,et al. Unlearning versus savings in visuomotor adaptation: comparing effects of washout, passage of time, and removal of errors on motor memory , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[42] P. Celnik,et al. Dissociating the roles of the cerebellum and motor cortex during adaptive learning: the motor cortex retains what the cerebellum learns. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.
[43] Nathaniel D. Daw,et al. Cognitive Control Predicts Use of Model-based Reinforcement Learning , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[44] Matthew J. Crossley,et al. Savings upon Re-Aiming in Visuomotor Adaptation , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[45] Reza Shadmehr,et al. Dissociable effects of the implicit and explicit memory systems on learning control of reaching , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[46] J. Rothwell,et al. The dissociable effects of punishment and reward on motor learning , 2015, Nature Neuroscience.
[47] J. Galea,et al. Individual differences in explicit and implicit visuomotor learning and working memory capacity , 2016, Scientific Reports.
[48] A. Markman,et al. The Curse of Planning: Dissecting Multiple Reinforcement-Learning Systems by Taxing the Central Executive , 2013 .
[49] Peter Dayan,et al. When Money Is Not Enough: Awareness, Success, and Variability in Motor Learning , 2014, PloS one.
[50] David M. Huberdeau,et al. The Influence of Movement Preparation Time on the Expression of Visuomotor Learning and Savings , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[51] P. Dayan,et al. Mapping value based planning and extensively trained choice in the human brain , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.
[52] Richard S. Sutton,et al. Integrated Architectures for Learning, Planning, and Reacting Based on Approximating Dynamic Programming , 1990, ML.
[53] Raymond J. Delnicki,et al. Overcoming Motor “Forgetting” Through Reinforcement Of Learned Actions , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[54] Philippe Lefèvre,et al. Overlap of movement planning and movement execution reduces reaction time by up to 100ms , 2016, bioRxiv.
[55] Alborz Geramifard,et al. Dyna-Style Planning with Linear Function Approximation and Prioritized Sweeping , 2008, UAI.
[56] A. Haith,et al. Model-based and model-free mechanisms of human motor learning. , 2013, Advances in experimental medicine and biology.
[57] J. Flanagan,et al. Relation between reaction time and reach errors during visuomotor adaptation , 2011, Behavioural Brain Research.
[58] R. Ivry,et al. Cerebellar and prefrontal cortex contributions to adaptation, strategies, and reinforcement learning. , 2014, Progress in brain research.
[59] Richard B. Ivry,et al. Flexible Cognitive Strategies during Motor Learning , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[60] J. Krakauer,et al. Sensory prediction errors drive cerebellum-dependent adaptation of reaching. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.
[61] Peter Holland,et al. Contribution of explicit processes to reinforcement-based motor learning. , 2018, Journal of neurophysiology.
[62] B. Balleine,et al. A specific role for posterior dorsolateral striatum in human habit learning , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.
[63] Stephen G Lisberger,et al. Role of Plasticity at Different Sites across the Time Course of Cerebellar Motor Learning , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[64] Daniel M. Wolpert,et al. Forward Models for Physiological Motor Control , 1996, Neural Networks.
[65] A. Haith,et al. Independence of Movement Preparation and Movement Initiation , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.