Attentional Factors in Conceptual Congruency
暂无分享,去创建一个
Marc Ouellet | Julio Santiago | Javier Valenzuela | Antonio Román | Javier Valenzuela | Julio Santiago | Antonio Román | Marc Ouellet
[1] Julio Santiago,et al. Flexible Conceptual Projection of Time Onto Spatial Frames of Reference , 2006, Cogn. Sci..
[2] D. Algom,et al. Size congruity effects with two-digit numbers: Expanding the number line? , 2006, Memory & cognition.
[3] Philippe Pinel,et al. Distributed and Overlapping Cerebral Representations of Number, Size, and Luminance during Comparative Judgments , 2004, Neuron.
[4] Seana Coulson,et al. Congruity Effects in Time and Space: Behavioral and ERP Measures , 2008, Cogn. Sci..
[5] Jean-Christophe Sarrazin,et al. Dynamics of balancing space and time in memory: tau and kappa effects revisited. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[6] G. Lakoff,et al. Metaphors We Live by , 1981 .
[7] Yoona Kang,et al. Physical temperature effects on trust behavior: the role of insula. , 2011, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.
[8] Michael L. Anderson. Neural reuse: A fundamental organizational principle of the brain , 2010, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[9] W. Levelt,et al. Speaking: From Intention to Articulation , 1990 .
[10] Chen-Bo Zhong,et al. Cold and Lonely , 2008, Psychological science.
[11] Thomas W. Schubert,et al. High in the Hierarchy: How Vertical Location and Judgments of Leaders' Power are Interrelated , 2007 .
[12] Dragan Vukmirovic,et al. Does Playing Blindfold Chess Reduce the Quality of Game: Comments on Chabris and Hearst (2003) , 2010, Cogn. Sci..
[13] M. Pesenti,et al. Numerosity-duration interference: a Stroop experiment. , 2006, Acta psychologica.
[14] Hans Ijzerman,et al. The Thermometer of Social Relations , 2009, Psychological science.
[15] Daniel Casasanto,et al. Do monkeys think in metaphors? Representations of space and time in monkeys and humans , 2010, Cognition.
[16] J. Pittenger,et al. Tau and Kappa effects in physical space: the case of audition , 2007, Psychological research.
[17] I.. THE ATTENTION SYSTEM OF THE HUMAN BRAIN , 2002 .
[18] H. Helson. THE TAU EFFECT--AN EXAMPLE OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RELATIVITY. , 1931, Science.
[19] Marc Ouellet,et al. In hindsight, life flows from left to right , 2010, Psychological research.
[20] Antonino Vallesi,et al. An effect of spatial–temporal association of response codes: Understanding the cognitive representations of time , 2008, Cognition.
[21] P. Rabbitt,et al. Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: time course of activation and resistance to interruption , 1989 .
[22] A. Henik,et al. Is three greater than five: The relation between physical and semantic size in comparison tasks , 1982, Memory & cognition.
[23] D. Casasanto,et al. of Experimental Psychology , 2022 .
[24] G. Murphy. On metaphoric representation , 1996, Cognition.
[25] D. Casasanto,et al. Similarity and proximity: When does close in space mean close in mind? , 2008, Memory & cognition.
[26] M. Corbetta,et al. The Reorienting System of the Human Brain: From Environment to Theory of Mind , 2008, Neuron.
[27] Daniel Casasanto,et al. Good and Bad in the Hands of Politicians: Spontaneous Gestures during Positive and Negative Speech , 2010, PloS one.
[28] L. Boroditsky. Metaphoric structuring: understanding time through spatial metaphors , 2000, Cognition.
[29] H. H. Clark. SPACE, TIME, SEMANTICS, AND THE CHILD , 1973 .
[30] D. Wentura,et al. Automatic vigilance: the attention-grabbing power of approach- and avoidance-related social information. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[31] Anja Ischebeck,et al. On the relative speed account of number-size interference in comparative judgments of numerals. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[32] Sheng He,et al. Larger stimuli are judged to last longer. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[33] A. Kiesel,et al. SNARC-like congruency based on number magnitude and response duration. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[34] F. Frassinetti,et al. Prismatic Lenses Shift Time Perception , 2009, Psychological science.
[35] J. Bargh,et al. Experiencing Physical Warmth Promotes Interpersonal Warmth , 2008, Science.
[36] S. Dehaene,et al. The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .
[37] A. Chatterjee,et al. Verbs, events and spatial representations , 1999, Neuropsychologia.
[38] D. Algom,et al. Stroop and Garner effects in comparative judgment of numerals: The role of attention. , 1999 .
[39] Marinella Cappelletti,et al. Dissociations and interactions between time, numerosity and space processing , 2009, Neuropsychologia.
[40] Daniel Casasanto,et al. Space and Time in the Child's Mind: Evidence for a Cross-Dimensional Asymmetry , 2010, Cogn. Sci..
[41] Thomas W. Schubert,et al. Spatial Dimensions of Social Thought , 2011 .
[42] Julio Santiago,et al. Time (also) flies from left to right , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[43] John A Bargh,et al. The Scaffolded Mind: Higher mental processes are grounded in early experience of the physical world. , 2009, European journal of social psychology.
[44] Thomas W. Schubert,et al. Control over the AssoCiAtion of Power And size , 2009 .
[45] John Cohen,et al. A New Phenomenon in Time Judgment , 1953, Nature.
[46] Daniel Casasanto,et al. Motor fluency shapes abstract concepts , 2010 .
[47] M. Posner,et al. Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[48] Derek Besner,et al. Ideographic and alphabetic processing in skilled reading of English , 1979, Neuropsychologia.
[49] E. Brannon,et al. Nonverbal representation of time and number in adults. , 2007, Acta psychologica.
[50] G. Murphy. Reasons to doubt the present evidence for metaphoric representation , 1997, Cognition.
[51] Aitao Lu,et al. Contextual effects on number–time interaction , 2009, Cognition.
[52] G. Miller,et al. Cognitive science. , 1981, Science.
[53] B. Tversky,et al. Cross-cultural and developmental trends in graphic productions , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.
[54] F. Strack,et al. Movement direction or change in distance? Self- and object-related approach–avoidance motions ☆ , 2008 .
[55] S. Chaiken,et al. Personality and Social Psychology Bulle- Tin Chen, Bargh / Consequences of Automatic Evaluation Immediate Behavioral Predispositions to Approach or Avoid the Stimulus , 2022 .
[56] Giacomo Koch,et al. Relativistic Compression and Expansion of Experiential Time in the Left and Right Space , 2008, PloS one.
[57] L. Boroditsky,et al. Time in the mind: Using space to think about time , 2008, Cognition.
[58] Simone Schnall,et al. With a Clean Conscience , 2008, Psychological science.
[59] K. M. Heilman,et al. Asyntactic Thematic Role Assignment: The Use of a Temporal-Spatial Strategy , 1995, Brain and Language.
[60] R. Blair,et al. Larger than Life: Humans' Nonverbal Status Cues Alter Perceived Size , 2009, PloS one.
[61] Vincent Walsh. A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[62] Marc Ouellet,et al. Thinking about the future moves attention to the right. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[63] Walter Schneider,et al. Micro Experimental Laboratory: An integrated system for IBM PC compatibles , 1988 .
[64] H. Kolk,et al. Agrammatic sentence production: Is verb second impaired in Dutch? , 2006, Brain and Language.
[65] Daniel Algom,et al. Comparative judgment of numerosity and numerical magnitude: attention preempts automaticity. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[66] B. Bergum,et al. Attention and performance IX , 1982 .
[67] Frédéric Alexandre,et al. A model of contextual effect on reproduced extents in recall tasks: the issue of the imputed motion hypothesis , 2005, Biological Cybernetics.
[68] G. Lakoff,et al. Philosophy in the flesh : the embodied mind and its challenge to Western thought , 1999 .
[69] G. Clore,et al. Do Approach-Avoidance Actions Create Attitudes? , 2006, Psychological science.
[70] Gün R. Semin,et al. Temperature perceptions as a ground for social proximity , 2010 .
[71] Michael D. Robinson,et al. When "light" and "dark" thoughts become light and dark responses: affect biases brightness judgments. , 2007, Emotion.
[72] Anne Maass,et al. Directional Bias in the Mental Representation of Spatial Events , 2003, Psychological science.
[73] Michael D. Robinson,et al. Why the Sunny Side Is Up , 2004, Psychological science.
[74] Bill Jones,et al. Space-time dependencies in psychophysical judgment of extent and duration: Algebraic models of the tau and kappa effects. , 1982 .
[75] Takahiro Kawabe,et al. Audiovisual tau effect. , 2008, Acta psychologica.
[76] Thomas W. Schubert,et al. Weight as an Embodiment of Importance , 2009, Psychological science.
[77] C. Caltagirone,et al. Perceiving numbers alters time perception , 2008, Neuroscience Letters.
[78] L. Thomas. The light and the dark , 1984, Nature.
[79] F. Moore. Cognitive development and the acquisition of language , 1973 .
[80] Brian Butterworth,et al. Time and numerosity estimation are independent: Behavioral evidence for two different systems using a conflict paradigm , 2010, Cognitive neuroscience.
[81] Orly Fuhrman,et al. Cross-Cultural Differences in Mental Representations of Time: Evidence From an Implicit Nonlinguistic Task , 2010, Cogn. Sci..
[82] Thomas W. Schubert. Your highness: vertical positions as perceptual symbols of power. , 2005, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[83] A. Markman,et al. Constraining Theories of Embodied Cognition , 2005, Psychological science.
[84] J. Cacioppo,et al. Rudimentary determinants of attitudes. II: Arm flexion and extension have differential effects on attitudes. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[85] Bruno L. Giordano,et al. Spatial representation of pitch height: the SMARC effect , 2006, Cognition.
[86] Giacomo Koch,et al. Spatial–temporal interactions in the human brain , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.
[87] K. Heilman,et al. Agrammatic Sentence Production: The Use of a Temporal-Spatial Strategy , 1995, Brain and Language.
[88] D. Casasanto,et al. Who's Afraid of the Big Bad Whorf? Crosslinguistic Differences in Temporal Language and Thought , 2008 .
[89] M. Tops,et al. Posing for success: Clenching a fist facilitates approach , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[90] O. John,et al. Automatic vigilance: the attention-grabbing power of negative social information. , 1991, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[91] D. Casasanto,et al. Motor action and emotional memory , 2010, Cognition.
[92] Max Weisbuch,et al. Tough and tender: embodied categorization of gender. , 2010, Psychological science.
[93] J. Jonides. Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind's eye's movement , 1981 .
[94] H. J. Muller,et al. Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: time course of activation and resistance to interruption. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[95] Michael D. Robinson,et al. Does "feeling down" mean seeing down? Depressive symptoms and vertical selective attention. , 2006 .
[96] Julio Santiago,et al. Flexible foundations of abstract thought: A review and a theory , 2011 .