Spontaneous eyeblinks during breaking continuous flash suppression are associated with increased detection times.

An eyeblink has a clear effect on low-level information processing because it temporarily occludes all visual information. Recent evidence suggests that eyeblinks can also modulate higher level processes (e.g., attentional resources), and vice versa. Despite these putative effects on different levels of information processing, eyeblinks are typically neglected in vision and in consciousness research. The main aim of this study was to investigate the timing and the effect of eyeblinks in an increasingly popular paradigm in consciousness research, namely breaking continuous flash suppression (b-CFS). Results show that participants generally refrain from blinking during a trial, that is, when they need to detect a suppressed stimulus. However, when they do blink during a trial, we observed a sharp increase in suppression time. This suggests that one needs to control for blinking when comparing detection times between conditions that could elicit phasic changes in blinking.

[1]  Jaeseung Jeong,et al.  Spontaneous Eyeblinks Are Correlated with Responses during the Stroop Task , 2012, PloS one.

[2]  Shigeru Kitazawa,et al.  Eyeblink entrainment at breakpoints of speech , 2010, Neuroscience Research.

[3]  Bernhard Hommel,et al.  Note Blinks of the Eye Predict Blinks of the Mind , 2022 .

[4]  G. Barbato,et al.  Diurnal variation in spontaneous eye-blink rate , 2000, Psychiatry Research.

[5]  P. Sterzer,et al.  Priming of object detection under continuous flash suppression depends on attention but not on part-whole configuration. , 2015, Journal of vision.

[6]  C. Karson Spontaneous eye-blink rates and dopaminergic systems. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[7]  Erik J. Sirevaag,et al.  MULTI-DIMENSIONAL CHARACTERIZATIONS OF OPERATOR STATE: A VALIDATION OF OCULOMOTOR METRICS , 1999 .

[8]  Marlies E. van Bochove,et al.  Blinking predicts enhanced cognitive control , 2012, Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience.

[9]  Axel Cleeremans,et al.  Different effects of executive and visuospatial working memory on visual consciousness , 2015, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[10]  J. L. Myers,et al.  Regression analyses of repeated measures data in cognitive research. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[11]  B. Hommel,et al.  More creative through positive mood? Not everyone! , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[12]  K. Fukuda,et al.  Analysis of Eyeblink Activity during Discriminative Tasks , 1994, Perceptual and motor skills.

[13]  Amitash Ojha,et al.  Modulation of resource allocation by intelligent individuals in linguistic, mathematical and visuo-spatial tasks. , 2015, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[14]  C. Koch,et al.  Differential processing of invisible congruent and incongruent scenes: a case for unconscious integration. , 2013, Journal of vision.

[15]  Cees van Leeuwen,et al.  Individual differences in perceptual switching rates; the role of occipital alpha and frontal theta band activity , 2005, Biological Cybernetics.

[16]  M. Aramideh,et al.  Eyelid movements: behavioral studies of blinking in humans under different stimulus conditions. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  Wim Fias,et al.  Correlation between individual differences in striatal dopamine and in visual consciousness , 2014, Current Biology.

[18]  T. Kahan,et al.  Identifying words that emerge into consciousness: Effects of word valence and unconscious previewing , 2015, Consciousness and Cognition.

[19]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[20]  Sheri L. Johnson,et al.  Spontaneous Eye-Blink Rate as an Index of Reward Responsivity , 2016, Clinical psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[21]  Chris L. E. Paffen,et al.  Breaking continuous flash suppression: competing for consciousness on the pre-semantic battlefield , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[22]  Alessandro Torcini,et al.  Clique of Functional Hubs Orchestrates Population Bursts in Developmentally Regulated Neural Networks , 2014, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[23]  J. Stern,et al.  Blinks as an index of cognitive activity during reading , 1991, Integrative physiological and behavioral science : the official journal of the Pavlovian Society.

[24]  Makoto Kato,et al.  Blink-related momentary activation of the default mode network while viewing videos , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[25]  Gary Lupyan,et al.  Language can boost otherwise unseen objects into visual awareness , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[26]  David A. Leopold,et al.  Stable perception of visually ambiguous patterns , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[27]  Su-Ling Yeh,et al.  Accessing the meaning of invisible words , 2011, Consciousness and Cognition.

[28]  C. Koch,et al.  Continuous flash suppression reduces negative afterimages , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[29]  R. T. Pivik,et al.  Endogenous eye blinks in preadolescents: relationship to information processing and performance , 2004, Biological Psychology.

[30]  K. Fukuda,et al.  Eye blinks: new indices for the detection of deception. , 2001, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[31]  Richard J. Davidson,et al.  PET Evidence for a Role for Striatal Dopamine in the Attentional Blink: Functional Implications , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[32]  Sheng He,et al.  Processing of Invisible Stimuli: Advantage of Upright Faces and Recognizable Words in Overcoming Interocular Suppression , 2007, Psychological science.

[33]  Denise C. Park,et al.  A lifespan database of adult facial stimuli , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[34]  J. P. Kalisvaart,et al.  Influence of Retinal Image Shifts and Extra-Retinal Eye Movement Signals on Binocular Rivalry Alternations , 2013, PloS one.

[35]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[36]  P. Sterzer,et al.  Breaking Continuous Flash Suppression: A New Measure of Unconscious Processing during Interocular Suppression? , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[37]  Stefan Van der Stigchel,et al.  Information Matching the Content of Visual Working Memory Is Prioritized for Conscious Access , 2013, Psychological science.

[38]  Keiichi Kitajo,et al.  Synchronization of spontaneous eyeblinks while viewing video stories , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[39]  Junji Ito,et al.  Perceptual Switching, Eye Movements, and the Bus Paradox , 2003, Perception.

[40]  S. Steinhauer,et al.  Blink before and after you think: blinks occur prior to and following cognitive load indexed by pupillary responses. , 2008, Psychophysiology.

[41]  Marius V Peelen,et al.  Interobject grouping facilitates visual awareness. , 2015, Journal of vision.

[42]  Hideki Ohira,et al.  Eyeblink Activity as an Index of Cognitive Processing: Temporal Distribution of Eyeblinks as an Indicator of Expectancy in Semantic Priming , 2004, Perceptual and motor skills.

[43]  R. Roth,et al.  In the Blink of an Eye: Relating Positive-Feedback Sensitivity to Striatal Dopamine D2-Like Receptors through Blink Rate , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  H. Ohira Eyeblink Activity in a Word-Naming Task as a Function of Semantic Priming and Cognitive Load , 1996, Perceptual and motor skills.