Contrôle sensori-moteur en situation de téléopération : données théoriques et perspectives ergonomiques
暂无分享,去创建一个
Carole Ferrel | I. Pennel | Yann Coello | J.-P. Orliaguet | Y. Coello | J. Orliaguet | I. Pennel | Carole Ferrel
[1] M. Goodale,et al. Visual control of reaching movements without vision of the limb , 1986, Experimental Brain Research.
[2] M. Jeannerod,et al. Limited conscious monitoring of motor performance in normal subjects , 1998, Neuropsychologia.
[3] C. C. A. M. Gielen,et al. Interactions between self-motion and depth perception in the processing of optic flow , 1996, Trends in Neurosciences.
[4] M. Jeannerod,et al. Temporal dissociation of motor responses and subjective awareness. A study in normal subjects. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[5] Albert Postma,et al. Egocentric and Exocentric Spatial Judgements of Visual Displacement , 1999 .
[6] John M. Foley,et al. Visually directed pointing as a function of target distance, direction, and available cues , 1972 .
[7] Yann Coello,et al. Determination of target distance in a structured environment: Selection of visual information for action , 2000 .
[8] G. Buess,et al. Robotics and systems technology for advanced endoscopic procedures: experiences in general surgery. , 1999, European journal of cardio-thoracic surgery : official journal of the European Association for Cardio-thoracic Surgery.
[9] P. Viviani,et al. Frames of reference and control parameters in visuomanual pointing. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[10] J. Wann,et al. Does limb proprioception drift? , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[11] G. Buess,et al. Robotics and telemanipulation technologies for endoscopic surgery , 2000, Surgical Endoscopy.
[12] James R. Tresilian,et al. Increasing confidence in vergence as a cue to distance , 1999, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[13] C Ghez,et al. Learning of scaling factors and reference axes for reaching movements. , 1996, Neuroreport.
[14] H. Cunningham. Aiming error under transformed spatial mappings suggests a structure for visual-motor maps. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[15] R B Welch,et al. Multiple concurrent visual-motor mappings: implications for models of adaptation. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[16] Otmar Bock,et al. Adaptation of aimed arm movements to sensorimotor discordance: evidence for direction-independent gain control , 1992, Behavioural Brain Research.
[17] D. Rosenbaum. Human movement initiation: specification of arm, direction, and extent. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[18] G. M. Redding,et al. Adaptive spatial alignment and strategic perceptual-motor control. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[19] C. Bard,et al. Reference systems for coding spatial information in normal subjects and a deafferented patient , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[20] Y. Rossetti,et al. The timing of color and location processing in the motor context , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.
[21] M. Jeannerod,et al. Selective perturbation of visual input during prehension movements , 1991, Experimental Brain Research.
[22] Y. Coello,et al. Pointing movement visually controlled through a video display: adaptation to scale change , 2000, Ergonomics.
[23] C. Prablanc,et al. Pointing movement in an artificial perturbing inertial field: A prospective paradigm for motor control study , 1996, Neuropsychologia.
[24] C. Prablanc,et al. Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement , 1986, Nature.
[25] B. Hannaford,et al. Force-feedback grasper helps restore sense of touch in minimally invasive surgery , 1999, Journal of Gastrointestinal Surgery.
[26] P. Fitts. The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.
[27] Michel Desmurget,et al. The effect of viewing the static hand prior to movement onset on pointing kinematics and variability , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.
[28] James Gordon,et al. Accuracy of planar reaching movements , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.
[29] H. Imamizu,et al. The locus of visual-motor learning at the task or manipulator level: implications from intermanual transfer. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[30] M. Kawato,et al. Internal representations of the motor apparatus: implications from generalization in visuomotor learning. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[31] Y. Coello,et al. Effect of Size and Frame of Visual Field on the Accuracy of an Aiming Movement , 1997, Perception.
[32] Martin Burghoff,et al. Visuo-motor adaptation: evidence for a distributed amplitude control system , 1997, Behavioural Brain Research.
[33] C. Bard,et al. Contribution of proprioception for calibrating and updating the motor space. , 1995, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.
[34] D. Pélisson,et al. From Eye to Hand: Planning Goal-directed Movements , 1998, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[35] Blake Hannaford,et al. Quantitative Evaluation of Perspective and Stereoscopic Displays in Three-Axis Manual Tracking Tasks , 1987, IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics.
[36] Rainer K. Bernotat,et al. Rotation of Visual Reference Systems and Its Influence on Control Quality , 1970 .
[37] M. Jeannerod,et al. Representation of hand position prior to movement and motor variability. , 1995, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.