Preventing Recovery From Extinction and Relapse

When a cue (X) and an outcome are paired, X comes to elicit responding similar to that elicited by the outcome. This potential is lessened if X is later presented by itself (i.e., experimental extinction). Extinction is interesting both for theoretical reasons and because it is a model for exposure therapy, which is used to treat numerous psychological disorders. Conventionally, extinction has been viewed as an isolated phenomenon unrelated to other response-attenuating treatments, whereas it is actually but one of several instances of associative interference that reduce conditioned responding. Although much has been learned about extinction from direct study, the larger associative-interference literature provides additional insight. Here we view extinction as new learning that is inconsistent with previously acquired knowledge (i.e., retroactive outcome interference). We provide an account of whether the acquisition or extinction memory will be expressed depending on the relative strengths of these two memories and the relative efficacies of their respective retrieval cues.

[1]  Steven M. Smith,et al.  Environmental context and human memory , 1978 .

[2]  Robert A. Bjork,et al.  A new theory of disuse and an old theory of stimulus fluctuation , 1992 .

[3]  M. Bouton,et al.  A retrieval cue for extinction attenuates spontaneous recovery. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[4]  M. Bouton Context, time, and memory retrieval in the interference paradigms of Pavlovian learning. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.

[5]  M. Bouton,et al.  A retrieval cue for extinction attenuates response recovery (renewal) caused by a return to the conditioning context. , 1994 .

[6]  Michael C. Anderson,et al.  Remembering can cause forgetting: retrieval dynamics in long-term memory. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[7]  R. R. Miller,et al.  Conducting exposure treatment in multiple contexts can prevent relapse. , 1998, Behaviour research and therapy.

[8]  Mark E. Bouton,et al.  Context change and retention interval can have additive, rather than interactive, effects after taste aversion extinction , 1998 .

[9]  R. R. Miller,et al.  Cues trained apart compete for behavioral control in rats: convergence with the associative interference literature. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[10]  Brian L. Thomas,et al.  Treatments that weaken Pavlovian conditioned fear and thwart its renewal in rats: implications for treating human phobias. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[11]  Ralph R. Miller,et al.  Associative interference between cues and between outcomes presented together and presented apart: an integration , 2002, Behavioural Processes.

[12]  Michael Davis,et al.  Facilitation of Conditioned Fear Extinction by Systemic Administration or Intra-Amygdala Infusions of d-Cycloserine as Assessed with Fear-Potentiated Startle in Rats , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  Effects of extinction context and retrieval cues on alcohol cue reactivity among nonalcoholic drinkers. , 2002 .

[14]  James C. Denniston,et al.  Massive extinction treatment attenuates the renewal effect , 2003 .

[15]  Gonzalo P Urcelay,et al.  Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery , 2009, Learning & behavior.

[16]  G. Urcelay,et al.  Constraints on Enhanced Extinction Resulting from Extinction Treatment in the Presence of an Added Excitor. , 2009, Learning and motivation.

[17]  M. Escobar,et al.  Long-term maintenance of immediate or delayed extinction is determined by the extinction-test interval. , 2010, Learning & memory.

[18]  Ralph R. Miller,et al.  Behavioral Techniques to Reduce Relapse After Exposure Therapy , 2011 .

[19]  T. Schachtman,et al.  Associative learning and conditioning theory : human and non-human applications , 2011 .