Species replacement reduces community participation in avian antipredator groups

In diverse communities, recognizing other species’ alarm signals is critical for evading predators. Recognition among community members is thought to build up predictably and quickly as individuals learn to associate previously unrecognized calls with the presence of a predator. Here, we use a natural range expansion in which a songbird species, the pied flycatcher (Ficedula hypoleuca), is being gradually displaced on a Baltic island by an ecologically similar congeneric, the collared flycatcher (Ficedula albicollis), to test this prediction. We conducted 2 field experiments to evaluate the abilities of both species to recruit local heterospecifics to antipredator groups, called mobs. First, we stimulated mobs by exposing breeding collared and pied pairs to taxidermied predators. Second, to isolate the effect of alarm call recognition from other potential confounds, such as behavioral cues or differences in the locations of pied and collared flycatcher nests, we broadcast alarm calls of both species in breeding areas and measured the responses of heterospecifics. We found that pied flycatcher pairs were more likely to attract heterospecifics than were collared flycatcher pairs. This difference is driven by weak responses of community members to collared flycatcher alarm calls: The alarm calls of the native pied flycatcher were much more likely to attract heterospecifics than those of the colonist, collared flycatchers. Our results show that subtle changes in species composition may have large, unpredictable consequences on community-wide communication. Because many avian breeding communities are heavily affected by predation, disturbed communication networks may, in turn, have cascading effects on community composition.

[1]  Robert D Magrath,et al.  A micro-geography of fear: learning to eavesdrop on alarm calls of neighbouring heterospecifics , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[2]  J. Hoogland,et al.  Risk in mobbing for solitary and colonial swallows , 1986, Animal Behaviour.

[3]  Toshitaka N. Suzuki Referential calls coordinate multi-species mobbing in a forest bird community , 2015, Journal of Ethology.

[4]  David Wheatcroft,et al.  Repetition rate of calls used in multiple contexts communicates presence of predators to nestlings and adult birds , 2015, Animal Behaviour.

[5]  A. Flasskamp The Adaptive Significance of Avian Mobbing V. An Experimental Test of the ‘Move On’ Hypothesis , 2010 .

[6]  H. Ellegren,et al.  Speciation in Ficedula flycatchers , 2010, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[7]  Graeme D Ruxton,et al.  Interspecific information transfer influences animal community structure. , 2010, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[8]  T. Sordahl The risks of avian mobbing and distraction behavior : an anecdotal review , 1990 .

[9]  P. Banks,et al.  Naïveté in novel ecological interactions: lessons from theory and experimental evidence , 2014, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.

[10]  A. Qvarnström,et al.  POSITIVE FEEDBACK BETWEEN ECOLOGICAL AND REPRODUCTIVE CHARACTER DISPLACEMENT IN A YOUNG AVIAN HYBRID ZONE , 2012, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.

[11]  Peter Marler,et al.  Specific Distinctiveness in the Communication Signals of Birds , 1957 .

[12]  D. Bates,et al.  Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4 , 2014, 1406.5823.

[13]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[14]  H. Gelter,et al.  Song differences between the Pied Flycatcher Ficedula hypoleuca, the Collared Flycatcher F. albicollis, and their hybrids , 1987 .

[15]  U. Ernst,et al.  Cultural Transmission of Enemy Recognition: One Function of Mobbing , 1978, Science.

[16]  Robert D Magrath,et al.  Recognition of other species' aerial alarm calls: speaking the same language or learning another? , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[17]  David Wheatcroft,et al.  Learning and signal copying facilitate communication among bird species , 2013, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[18]  Linking anti-predator behaviour and habitat quality: group effect in nest defence of a passerine bird , 2012, acta ethologica.

[19]  E. Curio Die akustische Wirkung von Feindalarmen auf einige Singvögel , 1971, Journal für Ornithologie.

[20]  J. Hare,et al.  The squirrel that cried wolf: reliability detection by juvenile Richardson's ground squirrels (Spermophilus richardsonii) , 2001, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[21]  W. Shriner Antipredator Responses to a Previously Neutral Sound by Free-living Adult Golden-mantled Ground Squirrels, Spermophilus lateralis (Sciuridae) , 1999 .

[22]  A. Qvarnström,et al.  Life-history divergence facilitates regional coexistence of competing Ficedula flycatchers. , 2009, Ecology.

[23]  Robert D. Magrath,et al.  Wild Birds Learn to Eavesdrop on Heterospecific Alarm Calls , 2015, Current Biology.

[24]  C. Both,et al.  Avian population consequences of climate change are most severe for long-distance migrants in seasonal habitats , 2010, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[25]  N. Davies,et al.  Social Transmission of a Host Defense Against Cuckoo Parasitism , 2009, Science.

[26]  T. Veen,et al.  Experimental evidence for innate predator recognition in the Seychelles warbler , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[27]  J. Gardner,et al.  Sound familiar? Acoustic similarity provokes responses to unfamiliar heterospecific alarm calls , 2011 .

[28]  G. Klump,et al.  Acoustic Behaviour of Birds and Mammals in the Predator Context; I. Factors Affecting the Structure of Alarm Signals. II. The Functional Significance and Evolution of Alarm Signals , 2010 .

[29]  David Wheatcroft,et al.  Rates of signal evolution are associated with the nature of interspecific communication , 2015 .

[30]  T. Valone From eavesdropping on performance to copying the behavior of others: a review of public information use , 2007, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[31]  R. Curry,et al.  ANTI-PREDATOR RESPONSES OF NEOTROPICAL RESIDENT AND MIGRANT BIRDS TO FAMILIAR AND UNFAMILIAR OWL VOCALIZATIONS ON THE YUCATAN PENINSULA , 2007 .

[32]  Per B. Brockhoff,et al.  lmerTest Package: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models , 2017 .

[33]  Erick Greene,et al.  Nuthatches eavesdrop on variations in heterospecific chickadee mobbing alarm calls , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[34]  T. Krama,et al.  Interspecific reciprocity explains mobbing behaviour of the breeding chaffinches, Fringilla coelebs , 2002, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[35]  C. Pavey,et al.  Effects of avian mobbing on roost use and diet of powerful owls, Ninox strenua , 1998, Animal Behaviour.

[36]  V. Devictor,et al.  Large-Scale Changes in Community Composition: Determining Land Use and Climate Change Signals , 2012, PloS one.

[37]  J. Suhonen,et al.  Effect of Willow Tit Poecile montanus alarm calls on attack rates by Pygmy Owls Glaucidium passerinum , 2013 .

[38]  I. Mclean,et al.  Historical and experimental learned predator recognition in free-living New-Zealand robins , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[39]  Christine R. Hurd Interspecific attraction to the mobbing calls of black-capped chickadees (Parus atricapillus) , 1996, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[40]  R. Seyfarth,et al.  The assessment by vervet monkeys of their own and another species' alarm calls , 1990, Animal Behaviour.

[41]  R. Bshary,et al.  Red colobus and Diana monkeys provide mutual protection against predators , 1997, Animal Behaviour.

[42]  T. Krama,et al.  The increased risk of predation enhances cooperation , 2010, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[43]  A. Radford,et al.  Eavesdropping on heterospecific alarm calls: from mechanisms to consequences , 2015, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.

[44]  S. L. Lima Predators and the breeding bird: behavioral and reproductive flexibility under the risk of predation , 2009, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.

[45]  Richard A. Pettifor,et al.  The effects of avian mobbing on a potential predator, the European kestrel, Falco tinnunculus , 1990, Animal Behaviour.

[46]  M. Griesser,et al.  Kinship modulates the attention of naïve individuals to the mobbing behaviour of role models , 2016, Animal Behaviour.