Are Arabic and verbal numbers processed in different ways?

Four experiments were conducted in order to examine effects of notation--Arabic and verbal numbers--on relevant and irrelevant numerical processing. In Experiment 1, notation interacted with the numerical distance effect, and irrelevant physical size affected numerical processing (i.e., size congruity effect) for both notations but to a lesser degree for verbal numbers. In contrast, size congruity had no effect when verbal numbers were the irrelevant dimension. In Experiments 2 and 3, different parameters that could possibly affect the results, such as discriminability and variability (Experiment 2) and the block design (Experiment 3), were controlled. The results replicated the effects obtained in Experiment 1. In Experiment 4, in which physical size was made more difficult to process, size congruity for irrelevant verbal numbers was observed. The present results imply that notation affects numerical processing and that Arabic and verbal numbers are represented separately, and thus it is suggested that current models of numerical processing should have separate comparison mechanisms for verbal and Arabic numbers.

[1]  G. Keppel Design and analysis: A researcher's handbook, 3rd ed. , 1991 .

[2]  M. H. Fischer,et al.  Do Negative Numbers Have a Place on the Mental Number Line , 2005 .

[3]  M. Brysbaert,et al.  Semantic priming in number naming , 2002, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[4]  S Dehaene,et al.  Attention, automaticity, and levels of representation in number processing. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[5]  H. Heinze,et al.  On the interaction of numerical and size information in digit comparison: a behavioral and event-related potential study , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  Avishai Henik,et al.  The Brain Locus of Interaction between Number and Size: A Combined Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging and Event-related Potential Study , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[7]  Jamie I. D. Campbell Architectures for numerical cognition , 1994, Cognition.

[8]  Anja Ischebeck,et al.  Differences between digit naming and number word reading in a flanker task , 2003, Memory & cognition.

[9]  Michael S. Gazzaniga,et al.  Numerical processing in the two hemispheres: Studies of a split-brain patient , 2005, Brain and Cognition.

[10]  L Girelli,et al.  The development of automaticity in accessing number magnitude. , 2000, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[11]  Dana Ganor-Stern,et al.  Automaticity in Processing Ordinal Information , 2004 .

[12]  Jamie I. D. Campbell,et al.  An encoding-complex approach to numerical cognition in Chinese-English bilinguals. , 2004, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[13]  N. Lavie Distracted and confused?: Selective attention under load , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[14]  Avishai Henik,et al.  Are numbers special? The comparison systems of the human brain investigated by fMRI , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  S. Dehaene,et al.  THREE PARIETAL CIRCUITS FOR NUMBER PROCESSING , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[16]  M. Brysbaert,et al.  Are Arabic numerals processed as pictures in a Stroop interference task? , 2001, Psychological research.

[17]  H. Wiese Iconic and non-iconic stages in number development: the role of language , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[18]  Derek Besner,et al.  Ideographic and alphabetic processing in skilled reading of English , 1979, Neuropsychologia.

[19]  E. M. Duncan,et al.  Isolating the effects of symbolic distance, and semantic congruity in comparative judgments: An additive-factors analysis , 1980, Memory & cognition.

[20]  Melissa E. Libertus,et al.  Electrophysiological evidence for notation independence in numerical processing , 2007, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[21]  A. Henik,et al.  Unintentional word reading via the phonological route: The Stroop effect with cross-script homophones. , 1996 .

[22]  R. Cohen Kadosh The laterality effect: myth or truth? , 2008, Consciousness and cognition.

[23]  W Fias,et al.  Two routes for the processing of verbal numbers: evidence from the SNARC effect , 2001, Psychological research.

[24]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Abstract representations of numbers in the animal and human brain , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.

[25]  A. Paivio Perceptual comparisons through the mind’s eye , 1975, Memory & cognition.

[26]  Anja Ischebeck,et al.  On the relative speed account of number-size interference in comparative judgments of numerals. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  Avishai Henik,et al.  The effect of orientation on number word processing. , 2007, Acta psychologica.

[28]  Kathrin Cohen Kadosh,et al.  Developing a cortex specialized for face perception , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[29]  J. Lammertyn,et al.  The hunt for SNARC , 2005 .

[30]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The priming method: imaging unconscious repetition priming reveals an abstract representation of number in the parietal lobes. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[31]  Avishai Henik,et al.  Notation-Dependent and -Independent Representations of Numbers in the Parietal Lobes , 2007, Neuron.

[32]  A. Henik,et al.  Is three greater than five: The relation between physical and semantic size in comparison tasks , 1982, Memory & cognition.

[33]  S. Dehaene Varieties of numerical abilities , 1992, Cognition.

[34]  Silke M. Göbel,et al.  Parietal rTMS distorts the mental number line: Simulating ‘spatial’ neglect in healthy subjects , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[35]  D. Algom,et al.  The perception of number from the separability of the stimulus: The Stroop effect revisited , 1996, Memory & cognition.

[36]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Unconscious semantic priming extends to novel unseen stimuli , 2001, Cognition.

[37]  Avishai Henik,et al.  Automatic and intentional processing of numerical information , 1992 .

[38]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  Cross-notation number priming investigated at different stimulus onset asynchronies in parity and naming tasks. , 2004, Experimental psychology.

[39]  Dana Ganor-Stern,et al.  Across-notation automatic numerical processing. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[40]  D. LeBihan,et al.  Modulation of Parietal Activation by Semantic Distance in a Number Comparison Task , 2001, NeuroImage.

[41]  S. Dehaene,et al.  A Magnitude Code Common to Numerosities and Number Symbols in Human Intraparietal Cortex , 2007, Neuron.

[42]  Avishai Henik,et al.  The development of internal representations of magnitude and their association with Arabic numerals. , 2002, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[43]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  The Organization of Brain Activations in Number Comparison: Event-Related Potentials and the Additive-Factors Method , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[44]  J. P. Morgan,et al.  Design and Analysis: A Researcher's Handbook , 2005, Technometrics.

[45]  Takeshi Hatta,et al.  Semantic processing of Arabic, Kanji, and Kana numbers: Evidence from interference in physical and numerical size judgments , 2003, Memory & cognition.

[46]  Jamie I. D. Campbell Handbook of mathematical cognition , 2004 .

[47]  Avishai Henik,et al.  A common representation for semantic and physical properties: a cognitive-anatomical approach. , 2006, Experimental psychology.

[48]  Roi Cohen Kadosh,et al.  Are numbers special? An overview of chronometric, neuroimaging, developmental and comparative studies of magnitude representation , 2008, Progress in Neurobiology.

[49]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Primed numbers : Exploring the modularity of numerical representations with masked and unmasked semantic priming , 1999 .

[50]  A. Henik,et al.  UNINTENTIONAL WORD READING VIA THE PHONOLOGICAL ROUTE : THE STROOP EFFECT WITH CROSS-SCRIPT HOMOPHONES , 1996 .

[51]  Roi Cohen Kadosh,et al.  Numerical representation : Abstract or nonabstract ? , 2008 .

[52]  G. R. Potts,et al.  Mental comparison of size and magnitude: size congruity effects. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[53]  D. Algom,et al.  Stroop and Garner effects in comparative judgment of numerals: The role of attention. , 1999 .

[54]  Samuel Shaki,et al.  On the mental representation of negative numbers: Context-dependent SNARC effects with comparative judgments , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[55]  Avishai Henik,et al.  Virtual Dyscalculia Induced by Parietal-Lobe TMS Impairs Automatic Magnitude Processing , 2007, Current Biology.

[56]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Degeneracy and cognitive anatomy , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[57]  Lawrence W Barsalou,et al.  Abstraction in perceptual symbol systems. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[58]  Michael I. Posner,et al.  Is Word Recognition Automatic? A Cognitive-Anatomical Approach , 1989, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[59]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .

[60]  W. Fias The Importance of Magnitude Information in Numerical Processing: Evidence from the SNARC Effect , 1996 .

[61]  W Fias,et al.  Irrelevant digits affect feature-based attention depending on the overlap of neural circuits. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.