An fMRI study of risk‐taking following wins and losses: Implications for the gambler's fallacy

Human decision‐making involving independent events is often biased and affected by prior outcomes. Using a controlled task that allows us to manipulate prior outcomes, the present study examined the effect of prior outcomes on subsequent decisions in a group of young adults. We found that participants were more risk‐seeking after losing a gamble (riskloss) than after winning a gamble (riskwin), a pattern resembling the gambler's fallacy. Functional MRI data revealed that decisions after riskloss were associated with increased activation in the frontoparietal network, but decreased activation in the caudate and ventral striatum. The increased risk‐seeking behavior after a loss showed a trend of positive correlation with activation in the frontoparietal network and the left lateral orbitofrontal cortex but a trend of negative correlation with activation in the amgydala and caudate. In addition, there was a trend of positive correlation between feedback‐related activation in the left lateral frontal cortex and subsequent increased risk‐seeking behavior. These results suggest that a strong cognitive control mechanism but a weak affective decision‐making and reinforcement learning mechanism that usually contribute to flexible, goal‐directed decisions can lead to decision biases involving random events. This has significant implications for our understanding of the gambler's fallacy and human decision making under risk. Hum Brain Mapp, 2011. © 2010 Wiley‐Liss, Inc.

[1]  M. Jenkinson Non-linear registration aka Spatial normalisation , 2007 .

[2]  Robert T. Knight,et al.  Making order from chaos: the misguided frontal lobe , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[3]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dissociable Neural Responses in Human Reward Systems , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  Insights into the ageing mind: a view from cognitive neuroscience , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[5]  R. Passingham,et al.  Knowing When to Stop: The Brain Mechanisms of Chasing Losses , 2008, Biological Psychiatry.

[6]  Christopher K. Hsee,et al.  Risk as Feelings , 2001, Psychological bulletin.

[7]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  A Parametric Study of Prefrontal Cortex Involvement in Human Working Memory , 1996, NeuroImage.

[8]  Sabrina M. Tom,et al.  The Neural Basis of Loss Aversion in Decision-Making Under Risk , 2007, Science.

[9]  N. Tarrier,et al.  Towards a Cognitive-Behavioural Theory of Problem Gambling , 1993, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[10]  B. Postle,et al.  Prefrontal cortical contributions to working memory: evidence from event-related fMRI studies , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  A. Bechara,et al.  Cerebral Cortex doi:10.1093/cercor/bhn147 Functional Dissociations of Risk and Reward Processing in the Medial Prefrontal Cortex , 2008 .

[12]  G Elliott Wimmer,et al.  Nucleus accumbens activation mediates the influence of reward cues on financial risk taking , 2008, Neuroreport.

[13]  L. Nystrom,et al.  Tracking the hemodynamic responses to reward and punishment in the striatum. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  Rachel T. A. Croson,et al.  The Gambler’s Fallacy and the Hot Hand: Empirical Data from Casinos , 2005 .

[15]  M. Rabin Inference by Believers in the Law of Small Numbers , 2000 .

[16]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Understanding decision-making deficits in neurological conditions: insights from models of natural action selection , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[17]  A. Damasio,et al.  Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex , 1994, Cognition.

[18]  M. Jenkinson,et al.  Non-linear optimisation FMRIB Technial Report TR 07 JA 1 , 2007 .

[19]  J. D. Nichols,et al.  Double dissociation of conditioning and declarative knowledge relative to the amygdala and hippocampus in humans. , 1997, Science.

[20]  Gregory P. Lee,et al.  Different Contributions of the Human Amygdala and Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex to Decision-Making , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dissociable Roles of Ventral and Dorsal Striatum in Instrumental Conditioning , 2004, Science.

[22]  L. Clark,et al.  Gambling Near-Misses Enhance Motivation to Gamble and Recruit Win-Related Brain Circuitry , 2009, Neuron.

[23]  M. Delgado,et al.  Modulation of Caudate Activity by Action Contingency , 2004, Neuron.

[24]  W. Schultz Getting Formal with Dopamine and Reward , 2002, Neuron.

[25]  E. Rolls,et al.  Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[26]  A. Bechara Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: a neurocognitive perspective , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[27]  E. Rolls The orbitofrontal cortex and reward. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[28]  P. Dayan,et al.  Cortical substrates for exploratory decisions in humans , 2006, Nature.

[29]  A. Damasio,et al.  Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[30]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Neural Substrates for Reversing Stimulus–Outcome and Stimulus–Response Associations , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Common neural substrates for inhibition of spoken and manual responses. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[32]  Irwin P. Levin,et al.  Age-related differences in adaptive decision making: Sensitivity to expected value in risky choice , 2007, Judgment and Decision Making.

[33]  Terrance Odean,et al.  Are Investors Reluctant to Realize Their Losses? , 1996 .

[34]  N. Sadato,et al.  Processing of Social and Monetary Rewards in the Human Striatum , 2008, Neuron.

[35]  Lana M Trick,et al.  Grouping and gambling: a Gestalt approach to understanding the gambler's fallacy. , 2003, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[36]  A. Tversky,et al.  The hot hand in basketball: On the misperception of random sequences , 1985, Cognitive Psychology.

[37]  Jan Derrfuss,et al.  Cognitive control in the posterior frontolateral cortex: evidence from common activations in task coordination, interference control, and working memory , 2004, NeuroImage.

[38]  Arthur W. Toga,et al.  Automatic independent component labeling for artifact removal in fMRI , 2008, NeuroImage.

[39]  Hanna Damasio,et al.  Investment Behavior and the Negative Side of Emotion , 2005, Psychological science.

[40]  H. Damasio,et al.  Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[41]  Cameron S Carter,et al.  Cognitive control involved in overcoming prepotent response tendencies and switching between tasks. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[42]  Mark W. Woolrich,et al.  Robust group analysis using outlier inference , 2008, NeuroImage.

[43]  J. Jonides,et al.  Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. , 1999, Science.

[44]  T. Robbins,et al.  Dissociation in prefrontal cortex of affective and attentional shifts , 1996, Nature.

[45]  M. Brass,et al.  Involvement of the inferior frontal junction in cognitive control: Meta‐analyses of switching and Stroop studies , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[46]  A. Damasio,et al.  Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[47]  T. Robbins,et al.  Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[48]  P. Glimcher,et al.  The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[49]  Antonio Damasio,et al.  The somatic marker hypothesis: A neural theory of economic decision , 2005, Games Econ. Behav..

[50]  Corianne Rogalsky,et al.  Increased activation in the right insula during risk-taking decision making is related to harm avoidance and neuroticism , 2003, NeuroImage.

[51]  Eliot Hazeltine,et al.  Dissociable Contributions of Prefrontal and Parietal Cortices to Response Selection , 2002, NeuroImage.

[52]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Perceiving patterns in random series: dynamic processing of sequence in prefrontal cortex , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[53]  A. Damasio,et al.  Deciding Advantageously Before Knowing the Advantageous Strategy , 1997, Science.

[54]  T. Robbins,et al.  Defining the Neural Mechanisms of Probabilistic Reversal Learning Using Event-Related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[55]  A M Dale,et al.  Optimal experimental design for event‐related fMRI , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[56]  A. Damasio,et al.  Role of the Amygdala in Decision‐Making , 2003, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[57]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images , 2001, Medical Image Anal..

[58]  Peter Ayton,et al.  The hot hand fallacy and the gambler’s fallacy: Two faces of subjective randomness? , 2004, Memory & cognition.

[59]  T. Robbins,et al.  Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[60]  Paul Slovic,et al.  Affect, risk, and decision making. , 2005, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.