Combination of attentional and spatial working memory deficits in Bálint–Holmes syndrome

This study aims to investigate whether attention and spatiotemporal integration deficits are dissociated in patients with bilateral posterior cortical atrophy (PCA), and whether it is their combination that leads to a severe clinical handicap. We recorded performance and ocular behavior of four PCA patients and four age‐matched controls in visual search and counting tasks. We measured the percentage of targets detected and the mean detection time in a “pop‐out” search. We also compared counting ability when a set of dots is presented briefly (in healthy individuals, the automatic deployment of attention over space allows a fast estimation of quantity) or for unlimited duration (favoring sequential counting, hence spatiotemporal integration). All patients showed reduced deployment of attention over space (simultanagnosia), resulting in increased visual search times and underestimations of the number of briefly presented dots. Only two patients showed ocular revisiting behavior that caused frequent omissions in visual search and overestimations of the number of dots presented for unlimited duration. The impairment to deploy attention is considered here as a bilateral covert attention deficit. Disorganized ocular exploration appears to be independent and is hypothesized to result from processes maintaining a salience map over time (spatial working memory) and especially across saccades.

[1]  M. Perenin,et al.  Optic ataxia: a specific disruption in visuomotor mechanisms. I. Different aspects of the deficit in reaching for objects. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[2]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Impaired Working Memory for Location but not for Colour or Shape in Visual Neglect: a Comparison of Parietal and Non-Parietal Lesions , 2004, Cortex.

[3]  C. Kennard,et al.  Impaired spatial working memory across saccades contributes to abnormal search in parietal neglect. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[4]  Hans-Otto Karnath,et al.  Restricted ocular exploration does not seem to explain simultanagnosia , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  A. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Induction of visual extinction by rapid‐rate transcranial magnetic stimulation of parietal lobe , 1994, Neurology.

[6]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Dissociable mechanisms of subitizing and counting: neuropsychological evidence from simultanagnosic patients. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  John Duncan,et al.  Attentional Functions in Dorsal and Ventral Simultanagnosia , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[8]  Z. Pylyshyn,et al.  What enumeration studies can show us about spatial attention: evidence for limited capacity preattentive processing. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  J. Bisley,et al.  Been there, seen that: a neural mechanism for performing efficient visual search. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  M. Husain,et al.  Rezsö Bálint and his most celebrated case. , 1988, Archives of neurology.

[11]  O. L. Zangwill,et al.  DISORDERS OF VISUAL SPACE PERCEPTION ASSOCIATED WITH LESIONS OF THE RIGHT CEREBRAL HEMISPHERE , 1944 .

[12]  C. Colby,et al.  Trans-saccadic perception , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[13]  Masud Husain,et al.  A deficit of spatial remapping in constructional apraxia after right-hemisphere stroke. , 2010, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[14]  L Pisella,et al.  Right-hemispheric dominance for visual remapping in humans , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[15]  I. Wolpert,et al.  Die simultanagnosie — störung der gesamtauffassung , 1924 .

[16]  J. Bisley,et al.  Microstimulation of posterior parietal cortex biases the selection of eye movement goals during search. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  Kathrin Finke,et al.  Spatial and non-spatial attention deficits in neurodegenerative diseases: assessment based on Bundesen's theory of visual attention (TVA). , 2006, Restorative neurology and neuroscience.

[18]  Jon Driver,et al.  Revisiting Previously Searched Locations in Visual Neglect: Role of Right Parietal and Frontal Lesions in Misjudging Old Locations as New , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  W. Heide,et al.  Cortical control of double‐step saccades: Implications for spatial orientation , 1995, Annals of neurology.

[20]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Saccadic dysmetria in a patient with a right frontoparietal lesion. The importance of corollary discharge for accurate spatial behaviour. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[21]  Giuseppe Vallar,et al.  Spatial Neglect, Balint-Homes' and Gerstmann's Syndrome, and Other Spatial Disorders , 2007, CNS Spectrums.

[22]  M. Husain,et al.  Role of right posterior parietal cortex in maintaining attention to spatial locations over time , 2009, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[23]  Benson Df,et al.  Posterior cortical atrophy. , 1988, Archives of neurology.

[24]  J. Harrison,et al.  Posterior cortical atrophy. , 1989, Clinical and experimental neurology.

[25]  Avishai Henik,et al.  Hemispheric Asymmetry in the Remapping and Maintenance of Visual Saliency Maps: A TMS Study , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  J Douglas Crawford,et al.  Cortical mechanisms for trans-saccadic memory and integration of multiple object features , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[27]  Laure Pisella,et al.  The contribution of spatial remapping impairments to unilateral visual neglect , 2004, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[28]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Neural basis and recovery of spatial attention deficits in spatial neglect , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[29]  Y. Rossetti,et al.  Systematic retinotopic reaching error vectors in unilateral optic ataxia , 2010, Cortex.

[30]  Paul M Bays,et al.  Spatial remapping of the visual world across saccades , 2007, Neuroreport.

[31]  Patrick Dupont,et al.  Lesion evidence for the critical role of the intraparietal sulcus in spatial attention. , 2011, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[32]  G Holmes,et al.  DISTURBANCES OF VISUAL ORIENTATION , 1918, The British journal of ophthalmology.

[33]  Chris Rorden,et al.  Non-spatially lateralized mechanisms in hemispatial neglect , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[34]  Jon Driver,et al.  Spatial working memory deficit in unilateral neglect , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[35]  J. Hodges,et al.  The topography of metabolic deficits in posterior cortical atrophy (the visual variant of Alzheimer’s disease) with FDG-PET , 2003, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[36]  A. Luria,et al.  Disorders of "simultaneous perception" in a case of bilateral occipito-parietal brain injury. , 1959, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[37]  R. Bálint Seelenlähmung des “Schauens”, optische Ataxie, räumliche Störung der Aufmerksamkeit. pp. 67–81 , 1909 .

[38]  James Danckert,et al.  Deficits in peripheral visual attention in patients with optic ataxia , 2007, Neuroreport.

[39]  J. Bisley,et al.  Evidence for differential top-down and bottom-up suppression in posterior parietal cortex , 2013, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[40]  Kirsten A. Dalrymple,et al.  Simulating simultanagnosia: spatially constricted vision mimics local capture and the global processing deficit , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[41]  Paul H. Garthwaite,et al.  Methods of testing for a deficit in single-case studies: Evaluation of statistical power by Monte Carlo simulation , 2006, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[42]  François Michel,et al.  Seeing without the Occipito-Parietal Cortex: Simultagnosia as a Shrinkage of the Attentional Visual Field , 2004, Behavioural neurology.