Motor Contagion during Human-Human and Human-Robot Interaction
暂无分享,去创建一个
G. Sandini | L. Fadiga | T. Pozzo | G. Metta | F. Nori | A. Sciutti | A. Bisio
[1] G. Sandini,et al. Robots can be perceived as goal-oriented agents , 2013 .
[2] Hermann J. Müller,et al. Does Observing Artificial Robotic Systems Influence Human Perceptual Processing in the Same Way as Observing Humans? , 2012, ICSR.
[3] T. Pozzo,et al. Motor resonance mechanisms are preserved in Alzheimer’s disease patients , 2012, Neuroscience.
[4] Emily S. Cross,et al. Robotic movement preferentially engages the action observation network , 2012, Human brain mapping.
[5] Umberto Castiello,et al. Motor cortex excitability is tightly coupled to observed movements , 2012, Neuropsychologia.
[6] Giulio Sandini,et al. Measuring Human-Robot Interaction Through Motor Resonance , 2012, Int. J. Soc. Robotics.
[7] Frank Broz,et al. Gaze in HRI: From modeling to communication , 2012, 2012 7th ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI).
[8] Umberto Castiello,et al. Grasping with tools: corticospinal excitability reflects observed hand movements. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.
[9] E Poliakoff,et al. How does visuomotor priming differ for biological and non-biological stimuli? A review of the evidence , 2012, Psychological Research.
[10] Umberto Castiello,et al. Corticospinal excitability modulation to hand muscles during the observation of appropriate versus inappropriate actions , 2011, Cognitive neuroscience.
[11] Clare Press,et al. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews Action Observation and Robotic Agents: Learning and Anthropomorphism , 2022 .
[12] V. Manera,et al. Cooperation or competition? Discriminating between social intentions by observing prehensile movements , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.
[13] N. Sebanz,et al. Psychological research on joint action: Theory and data , 2011 .
[14] C. Heyes. Automatic imitation. , 2011, Psychological bulletin.
[15] Alois Knoll,et al. Biological movement increases acceptance of humanoid robots as human partners in motor interaction , 2011, AI & SOCIETY.
[16] L. Fadiga,et al. Automatic versus Voluntary Motor Imitation: Effect of Visual Context and Stimulus Velocity , 2010, PloS one.
[17] Giulio Sandini,et al. The iCub humanoid robot: An open-systems platform for research in cognitive development , 2010, Neural Networks.
[18] U. Castiello,et al. Modulation of the action control system by social intention: unexpected social requests override preplanned action. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[19] G. Cheng,et al. Social cognitive neuroscience and humanoid robotics , 2009, Journal of Physiology-Paris.
[20] Katsumi Watanabe,et al. Behavioral speed contagion: Automatic modulation of movement timing by observation of body movements , 2008, Cognition.
[21] A. Schnitzler,et al. Do simple intransitive finger movements consistently activate frontoparietal mirror neuron areas in humans? , 2007, NeuroImage.
[22] Vilayanur S. Ramachandran,et al. EEG evidence for mirror neuron activity during the observation of human and robot actions: Toward an analysis of the human qualities of interactive robots , 2007, Neurocomputing.
[23] Scott T. Grafton,et al. Evidence for a distributed hierarchy of action representation in the brain. , 2007, Human movement science.
[24] A. Hamilton,et al. Interference effect of observed human movement on action is due to velocity profile of biological motion , 2007, Social neuroscience.
[25] Christian Keysers,et al. The anthropomorphic brain: The mirror neuron system responds to human and robotic actions , 2007, NeuroImage.
[26] M. Shiffrar,et al. Fitts's Law Holds for Action Perception , 2007, Psychological science.
[27] T. Pozzo,et al. Kinematic features of movement tunes perception and action coupling , 2006, Behavioural Brain Research.
[28] C. Heyes,et al. Robotic movement elicits automatic imitation. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[29] Uta Frith,et al. Theory of mind , 2001, Current Biology.
[30] R. Rumiati,et al. The strategic control of multiple routes in imitation of actions. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[31] G. Rizzolatti,et al. The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.
[32] U. Castiello,et al. The Human Premotor Cortex Is 'Mirror' Only for Biological Actions , 2004, Current Biology.
[33] A. van Knippenberg,et al. Mimicry and Prosocial Behavior , 2004, Psychological science.
[34] T. Chartrand,et al. Using Nonconscious Behavioral Mimicry to Create Affiliation and Rapport , 2003, Psychological science.
[35] U. Castiello. Understanding other people's actions: intention and attention. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[36] H. Bekkering,et al. Action generation and action perception in imitation: an instance of the ideomotor principle. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[37] Y. Paulignan,et al. An Interference Effect of Observed Biological Movement on Action , 2003, Current Biology.
[38] C. Frith,et al. Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’ , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[39] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. Observing a human or a robotic hand grasping an object: Differential motor priming effects , 2002 .
[40] W. Prinz,et al. Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. , 2001, Acta psychologica.
[41] H. Bekkering,et al. Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[42] C. Frith,et al. Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks , 2000, Neuropsychologia.
[43] J. Mazziotta,et al. Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. , 1999, Science.
[44] T. Chartrand,et al. The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[45] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Resonance behaviors and mirror neurons. , 1999, Archives italiennes de biologie.
[46] A. Premack,et al. Infants Attribute Value to the Goal-Directed Actions of Self-propelled Objects , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[47] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Action recognition in the premotor cortex. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[48] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[49] A. Karmiloff-Smith,et al. Are children with autism blind to the mentalistic significance of the eyes , 1995 .