Effect of Loading Symbol of Online Video on Perception of Waiting Time

ABSTRACT The aim of this study was to investigate the effect of different loading symbols and loading durations on online video viewers’ perception of waiting time. Sixty young adults participated in this study and their subjective ratings on waiting time perception for forty-eight different loading symbols (4 progress functions × 2 shapes × 2 embellishments × 3 durations) were obtained through a 7-point Likert scale. Each participant chose the best loading symbols in terms of waiting time perception at the end of the experiment. Results showed that duration and the progress function practically affect the viewers’ waiting time perception, and shape and embellishment do not. Loading symbols of power and inverse power progress functions are perceived as shorter than those of the repetitive and linear progress functions. It is recommended to show loading progress and use manipulated progress functions, and design factors such as shape and embellishment are considered to be less important for designing a better loading symbol. The findings of this study may serve as a useful input for video service providers and symbol designers in creating shortly perceived loading symbols.

[1]  C I HOVLAND,et al.  Assimilation and contrast effects of anchoring stimuli on judgments. , 1958, Journal of experimental psychology.

[2]  Avi Parush,et al.  The Impact of Visual Layout Factors on Performance in Web Pages: A Cross-Language Study , 2005, Hum. Factors.

[3]  R. E. Hicks,et al.  Transfer of time judgments as a function of feedback. , 1976, The American journal of psychology.

[4]  Toon De Pessemier,et al.  Quantifying Subjective Quality Evaluations for Mobile Video Watching in a Semi-Living Lab Context , 2012, IEEE Transactions on Broadcasting.

[5]  Barrett S. Caldwell,et al.  Factors affecting user tolerance for voice mail message transmission delays , 1995, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Interact..

[6]  Hogan Hw,et al.  A theoretical reconciliation of competing views of time perception. , 1978 .

[7]  Alex Paul Conn,et al.  Time affordances: the time factor in diagnostic usability heuristics , 1995, CHI '95.

[8]  Brad A. Myers,et al.  The importance of percent-done progress indicators for computer-human interfaces , 1985, CHI '85.

[9]  Peter Jones,et al.  Managing perceptions of waiting times in service queues , 1996 .

[10]  A. Pruyn,et al.  Effects of waiting on the satisfaction with the service: Beyond objective time measures , 1998 .

[11]  Lassi A. Liikkanen,et al.  Designing interactive systems for the experience of time , 2013, DPPI.

[12]  Detmar W. Straub,et al.  Culture and consumer responses to web download time: a four-continent study of mono and polychronism , 2003, IEEE Trans. Engineering Management.

[13]  Bruce Tog Tognazzini,et al.  Principles, techniques, and ethics of stage magic and their application to human interface design , 1993, INTERCHI.

[14]  Ewart A. C. Thomas,et al.  Cognitive processing and time perception , 1975 .

[15]  Alistair Sutcliff,et al.  Designing interactive systems , 1996, SGCH.

[16]  Helga Lejeune,et al.  Vierordt's The Experimental Study of the Time Sense (1868) and its legacy , 2009 .

[17]  P. Lachenbruch Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed.) , 1989 .

[18]  M. Treisman Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval. Implications for a model of the "internal clock". , 1963, Psychological monographs.

[19]  Jean-Charles Chebat,et al.  The Impact of Waiting in Line on Consumers , 1993 .

[20]  A. Angrilli,et al.  The influence of affective factors on time perception , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[21]  Cees J. H. Midden,et al.  Monitoring and Evaluation of Time Delay , 2005, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Interact..

[22]  Leif D. Nelson,et al.  You’re Having Fun When Time Flies , 2010, Psychological science.

[23]  Roland Thomaschke,et al.  Predictivity of system delays shortens human response time , 2014, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[24]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Duration neglect in retrospective evaluations of affective episodes. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[25]  Fiona Fui-Hoon Nah,et al.  A study on tolerable waiting time: how long are Web users willing to wait? , 2004, AMCIS.

[26]  Alan Clarke Use of graphics , 2018 .

[27]  John J. McCann,et al.  Assimiliation and Contrast , 2001, CIC.

[28]  André J. Szameitat,et al.  Behavioral and emotional consequences of brief delays in human-computer interaction , 2009, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[29]  Ramesh K. Sitaraman,et al.  Video Stream Quality Impacts Viewer Behavior: Inferring Causality Using Quasi-Experimental Designs , 2013, IEEE/ACM Transactions on Networking.

[30]  Richard A. Block,et al.  Cognitive models of psychological time , 1992 .

[31]  Osamu Iwata Basic Studies on Intolerance forWaiting-Stress , 1994 .

[32]  Laura Schweitzer,et al.  Wait times, patient satisfaction scores, and the perception of care. , 2014, The American journal of managed care.

[33]  Chung-Hsing Yeh,et al.  How will the use of graphics affect visual aesthetics? A user-centered approach for web page design , 2013, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[34]  Zhiquan Yeo,et al.  Faster progress bars: manipulating perceived duration with visual augmentations , 2010, CHI.

[35]  F. Macar,et al.  Controlled attention sharing influences time estimation , 1994, Memory & cognition.

[36]  Nik Kamariah Nik Mat,et al.  The Effects of Waiting Time, Passive and Active Impatience on Anger and Waiting Acceptability , 2012 .

[37]  P R Yarnold,et al.  Effects of actual waiting time, perceived waiting time, information delivery, and expressive quality on patient satisfaction in the emergency department. , 1996, Annals of emergency medicine.

[38]  Michael S. Borella,et al.  The effect of network delay and media on user perceptions of web resources , 2000, Behav. Inf. Technol..

[39]  Robert Bell,et al.  Rethinking the progress bar , 2007, UIST.

[40]  Othman Yeop The Effects of Waiting Time, Passive and Active Impatience on Anger and Waiting Acceptability , 2012 .

[41]  W. Boucsein,et al.  Standardized task strain and system response times in human-computer interaction. , 1995, Ergonomics.

[42]  Jacob Cohen Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[43]  Ramesh K. Sitaraman,et al.  Video Stream Quality Impacts Viewer Behavior: Inferring Causality Using Quasi-Experimental Designs , 2012, IEEE/ACM Transactions on Networking.

[44]  Ethan V. Munson,et al.  40 Years of Searching for the Best Computer System Response Time , 2011, Interact. Comput..

[45]  Ben Shneiderman,et al.  Determining Causes and Severity of End-User Frustration , 2004, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Interact..

[46]  Yehoshua Tsal,et al.  Awareness of attention allocation and time estimation accuracy , 1989 .

[47]  D Shinar,et al.  Duration estimates and users' preferences in human-computer interaction. , 1996, Ergonomics.

[48]  Grey Giddins,et al.  Statistics , 2016, The Journal of hand surgery, European volume.

[49]  D. Zakay Attention allocation policy influences prospective timing , 1998 .

[50]  Satoshi Uemura,et al.  User study of the subjective quality of mobile streaming service , 2012, Mobile HCI.

[51]  S. Grondin Timing and time perception: A review of recent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoretical directions , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[52]  Christopher A. Sanchez,et al.  Feedback Preferences and Impressions of Waiting , 2009, Hum. Factors.

[53]  L. Rüschendorf,et al.  On the Perception of Time , 2009, Gerontology.

[54]  H W Hogan A theoretical reconciliation of competing views of time perception. , 1978, The American journal of psychology.