How Learning to Read Changes the Cortical Networks for Vision and Language

Reading, Writing, and Face Recognition Reading, not to mention writing and texting, is a relatively recent invention, and hence it is believed that a preliterate brain must adapt on the fly, so to speak, in learning how to process written words, rather than being able to rely upon evolutionarily ancient modifications of the visual system pathways. Dehaene et al. (p. 1359, published online 11 November) examined the neural response to a range of visual stimuli in three groups: illiterate adults, adults who learned to read as children, and adults who learned to read as adults. Reading induced a greater facility in processing horizontally oriented stimuli at early stages in the visual pathway and was also associated with the appearance of an area specialized for words. This gain of function appeared to occur at a cost—the area in the temporal cortex devoted to face processing shrank. Reading changes the mind. Does literacy improve brain function? Does it also entail losses? Using functional magnetic resonance imaging, we measured brain responses to spoken and written language, visual faces, houses, tools, and checkers in adults of variable literacy (10 were illiterate, 22 became literate as adults, and 31 were literate in childhood). As literacy enhanced the left fusiform activation evoked by writing, it induced a small competition with faces at this location, but also broadly enhanced visual responses in fusiform and occipital cortex, extending to area V1. Literacy also enhanced phonological activation to speech in the planum temporale and afforded a top-down activation of orthography from spoken inputs. Most changes occurred even when literacy was acquired in adulthood, emphasizing that both childhood and adult education can profoundly refine cortical organization.

[1]  C. B. Colby The weirdest people in the world , 1973 .

[2]  J. Winn,et al.  Brain , 1878, The Lancet.

[3]  P. Bertelson,et al.  Literacy training and speech segmentation , 1986, Cognition.

[4]  F. Grosjean Language and Cognitive Processes , 1996 .

[5]  M Ingvar,et al.  The illiterate brain. Learning to read and write during childhood influences the functional organization of the adult brain. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[6]  K. Rayner Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. , 1998, Psychological bulletin.

[7]  A. Castro-Caldas,et al.  Influence of learning to read and write on the morphology of the corpus callosum , 1999, European journal of neurology.

[8]  Karl Magnus Petersson,et al.  Effective Auditory–Verbal Encoding Activates the Left Prefrontal and the Medial Temporal Lobes: A Generalization to Illiterate Subjects , 1999, NeuroImage.

[9]  Karl Magnus Petersson,et al.  Language Processing Modulated by Literacy: A Network Analysis of Verbal Repetition in Literate and Illiterate Subjects , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[10]  D. Wilkin,et al.  Neuron , 2001, Brain Research.

[11]  P. Skudlarski,et al.  Disruption of posterior brain systems for reading in children with developmental dyslexia , 2002, Biological Psychiatry.

[12]  I. Gauthier,et al.  Perceptual interference supports a non-modular account of face processing , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  Jean-Luc Velay,et al.  Visual presentation of single letters activates a premotor area involved in writing , 2003, NeuroImage.

[14]  Joel R. Meyer,et al.  Relation between brain activation and lexical performance , 2003, Human brain mapping.

[15]  D. LeBihan,et al.  Phonological Grammar Shapes the Auditory Cortex: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  Thomas A Zeffiro,et al.  Development of neural mechanisms for reading , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[17]  M. Tarr,et al.  Visual expertise with nonface objects leads to competition with the early perceptual processing of faces in the human occipitotemporal cortex. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[18]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Distinct unimodal and multimodal regions for word processing in the left temporal cortex , 2004, NeuroImage.

[19]  T. Nazir,et al.  Reading habits, perceptual learning, and recognition of printed words , 2004, Brain and Language.

[20]  C. Gilbert,et al.  Perceptual learning and top-down influences in primary visual cortex , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  R. Goebel,et al.  Integration of Letters and Speech Sounds in the Human Brain , 2004, Neuron.

[22]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Specialization within the ventral stream: the case for the visual word form area , 2004, NeuroImage.

[23]  H. Cheung,et al.  Early orthographic experience modifies both phonological awareness and on-line speech processing , 2004 .

[24]  W. Schneider,et al.  Cross‐cultural effect on the brain revisited: Universal structures plus writing system variation , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[25]  Raymond J. Dolan,et al.  Familiarity enhances invariance of face representations in human ventral visual cortex: fMRI evidence , 2005, NeuroImage.

[26]  宁北芳,et al.  疟原虫var基因转换速率变化导致抗原变异[英]/Paul H, Robert P, Christodoulou Z, et al//Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A , 2005 .

[27]  C. Gilbert,et al.  Top-Down Reorganization of Activity in the Visual Pathway after Learning a Shape Identification Task , 2005, Neuron.

[28]  Peter T Fox,et al.  Cognitive processing in Chinese literate and illiterate subjects: An fMRI study , 2006, Human brain mapping.

[29]  Michael M Merzenich,et al.  Perceptual Learning Directs Auditory Cortical Map Reorganization through Top-Down Influences , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Domain specificity in visual cortex. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[31]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Direct Intracranial, fMRI, and Lesion Evidence for the Causal Role of Left Inferotemporal Cortex in Reading , 2006, Neuron.

[32]  Daniel Brandeis,et al.  Coarse neural tuning for print peaks when children learn to read , 2006, NeuroImage.

[33]  Régine Kolinsky,et al.  The development of the orthographic consistency effect in speech recognition: From sublexical to lexical involvement , 2007, Cognition.

[34]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Visual word processing and experiential origins of functional selectivity in human extrastriate cortex , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[35]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  Differential development of high-level visual cortex correlates with category-specific recognition memory , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[36]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Cultural Recycling of Cortical Maps , 2007, Neuron.

[37]  K. Petersson,et al.  Literacy: a cultural influence on functional left–right differences in the inferior parietal cortex , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.

[38]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Reading normal and degraded words: Contribution of the dorsal and ventral visual pathways , 2008, NeuroImage.

[39]  E. Vul,et al.  Begging the Question: The Nonindependence Error in fMRI Data Analysis , 2010 .

[40]  J. Ziegler,et al.  Feedback consistency effects in visual and auditory word recognition: where do we stand after more than a decade? , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[41]  Johannes C. Ziegler,et al.  Orthographic effects in spoken language: On-line activation or phonological restructuring? , 2009, Brain Research.

[42]  M. Seghier,et al.  An anatomical signature for literacy , 2009, Nature.

[43]  R. Rosenfeld Nature , 2009, Otolaryngology--head and neck surgery : official journal of American Academy of Otolaryngology-Head and Neck Surgery.

[44]  A. Wenger,et al.  Reading in the brain , 2010 .

[45]  Urs Maurer,et al.  Auditory Selective Attention to Speech Modulates Activity in the Visual Word Form Area , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[46]  S. Levinson,et al.  WEIRD languages have misled us, too , 2010, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[47]  James R. Booth,et al.  Children with reading difficulties show differences in brain regions associated with orthographic processing during spoken language processing , 2010, Brain Research.

[48]  R. Goebel,et al.  Deviant processing of letters and speech sounds as proximate cause of reading failure: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of dyslexic children. , 2010, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[49]  H. Lyytinen,et al.  Brain sensitivity to print emerges when children learn letter–speech sound correspondences , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[50]  R. Malach,et al.  Top-down engagement modulates the neural expressions of visual expertise. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[51]  Ferath Kherif,et al.  Automatic Top-Down Processing Explains Common Left Occipito-Temporal Responses to Visual Words and Objects , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[52]  Masae Sato,et al.  Reading in the brain , 2012 .