Enhancing multiple object tracking performance with noninvasive brain stimulation: a causal role for the anterior intraparietal sulcus

Multiple object tracking (MOT) is a complex task recruiting a distributed network of brain regions. There are also marked individual differences in MOT performance. A positive causal relationship between the anterior intraparietal sulcus (AIPS), an integral region in the MOT attention network and inter-individual variation in MOT performance has not been previously established. The present study used transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS), a form of non-invasive brain stimulation, in order to examine such a causal link. Active anodal stimulation was applied to the right AIPS and the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) (and sham stimulation), an area associated with working memory (but not MOT) while participants completed a MOT task. Stimulation to the right AIPS significantly improved MOT accuracy more than the other two conditions. The results confirm a causal role of the AIPS in the MOT task and illustrate that tDCS has the ability to improve MOT performance.

[1]  Raja Parasuraman,et al.  Reducing the Disruptive Effects of Interruptions With Noninvasive Brain Stimulation , 2015, Hum. Factors.

[2]  Cyrus K. Foroughi,et al.  Activation and inhibition of posterior parietal cortex have bi-directional effects on spatial errors following interruptions , 2015, Front. Syst. Neurosci..

[3]  M. Bikson,et al.  Transcranial direct current stimulation facilitates cognitive multi-task performance differentially depending on anode location and subtask , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[4]  Dwight J. Peterson,et al.  Hits and misses: leveraging tDCS to advance cognitive research , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[5]  Raja Parasuraman,et al.  Battery powered thought: Enhancement of attention, learning, and memory in healthy adults using transcranial direct current stimulation , 2014, NeuroImage.

[6]  R Andy McKinley,et al.  Acceleration of image analyst training with transcranial direct current stimulation. , 2013, Behavioral neuroscience.

[7]  L. Parra,et al.  Cellular effects of acute direct current stimulation: somatic and synaptic terminal effects , 2013, The Journal of physiology.

[8]  G. Fink,et al.  Modulation of Top-Down Control of Visual Attention by Cathodal tDCS over Right IPS , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  M. Berryhill,et al.  Parietal Contributions to Visual Working Memory Depend on Task Difficulty , 2012, Front. Psychiatry.

[10]  O. Tzeng,et al.  Unleashing Potential: Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation over the Right Posterior Parietal Cortex Improves Change Detection in Low-Performing Individuals , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  Raja Parasuraman,et al.  Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Augments Perceptual Sensitivity and 24-Hour Retention in a Complex Threat Detection Task , 2012, PloS one.

[12]  Vince D. Calhoun,et al.  TDCS guided using fMRI significantly accelerates learning to identify concealed objects , 2012, NeuroImage.

[13]  A. A. Vakhtin,et al.  Transcranial direct current stimulation’s effect on novice versus experienced learning , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  Susanne M. Jaeggi,et al.  Short- and long-term benefits of cognitive training , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[15]  P. Enticott,et al.  Improving working memory: the effect of combining cognitive activity and anodal transcranial direct current stimulation to the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex , 2011, Brain Stimulation.

[16]  Todd S Horowitz,et al.  Direction information in multiple object tracking is limited by a graded resource , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[17]  U. Lindenberger,et al.  Hundred Days of Cognitive Training Enhance Broad Cognitive Abilities in Adulthood: Findings from the COGITO Study , 2010, Front. Ag. Neurosci..

[18]  Abhishek Datta,et al.  One-dimensional representation of a neuron in a uniform electric field , 2009, 2009 Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society.

[19]  George A. Alvarez,et al.  The Role of the Parietal Lobe in Visual Extinction Studied with Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[20]  Abhishek Datta,et al.  Establishing safety limits for transcranial direct current stimulation , 2009, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[21]  J. Wolfe,et al.  Using Fmri to Distinguish Components of the Multiple Object Tracking Task , 1994 .

[22]  A. Kramer,et al.  Can training in a real-time strategy video game attenuate cognitive decline in older adults? , 2008, Psychology and aging.

[23]  D. Simons,et al.  The effects of video game playing on attention, memory, and executive control. , 2008, Acta psychologica.

[24]  George A Alvarez,et al.  How many objects can you track? Evidence for a resource-limited attentive tracking mechanism. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[25]  J. Pratt,et al.  Playing an Action Video Game Reduces Gender Differences in Spatial Cognition , 2007, Psychological science.

[26]  Lesley A Ross,et al.  The Impact of Speed of Processing Training on Cognitive and Everyday Functions. , 2007, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[27]  J. Wolfe,et al.  Tracking unique objects , 2007, Perception & psychophysics.

[28]  D. Burr,et al.  Spatiotopic selectivity of BOLD responses to visual motion in human area MT , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[29]  C. S. Green,et al.  Enumeration versus multiple object tracking: the case of action video game players , 2006, Cognition.

[30]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Tracking multiple targets with multifocal attention , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[31]  D L Roenker,et al.  The impact of speed of processing training on cognitive and everyday performance , 2005, Aging & mental health.

[32]  J. Hyönä,et al.  Is multiple object tracking carried out automatically by an early vision mechanism independent of higher‐order cognition? An individual difference approach , 2004 .

[33]  J. Jefferys,et al.  Effects of uniform extracellular DC electric fields on excitability in rat hippocampal slices in vitro , 2004, The Journal of physiology.

[34]  Patrick Cavanagh,et al.  Unilateral Right Parietal Damage Leads to Bilateral Deficit for High-Level Motion , 2001, Neuron.

[35]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Attention Response Functions Characterizing Brain Areas Using fMRI Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load , 2001, Neuron.

[36]  M. Nitsche,et al.  External modulation of visual perception in humans , 2001, Neuroreport.

[37]  C. Koch,et al.  Brain Areas Specific for Attentional Load in a Motion-Tracking Task , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[38]  Z. Pylyshyn,et al.  What is a visual object? Evidence from target merging in multiple object tracking , 2001, Cognition.

[39]  A. Villringer,et al.  Involvement of the human frontal eye field and multiple parietal areas in covert visual selection during conjunction search , 2000, The European journal of neuroscience.

[40]  Z. Pylyshyn,et al.  Multiple object tracking and attentional processing. , 2000, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[41]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Cortical fMRI activation produced by attentive tracking of moving targets. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Functional localization of the system for visuospatial attention using positron emission tomography. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[43]  S. Yantis Multielement visual tracking: Attention and perceptual organization , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.

[44]  Z W Pylyshyn,et al.  Tracking multiple independent targets: evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism. , 1988, Spatial vision.

[45]  L. Bindman,et al.  The action of brief polarizing currents on the cerebral cortex of the rat (1) during current flow and (2) in the production of long‐lasting after‐effects , 1964, The Journal of physiology.