Specialization within the ventral stream: the case for the visual word form area
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] C. J. Price,et al. Cortical localisation of the visual and auditory word form areas: A reconsideration of the evidence , 2003, Brain and Language.
[2] R. Desimone,et al. Clustering of perirhinal neurons with similar properties following visual experience in adult monkeys , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[3] S. Dehaene,et al. The visual word form area: a prelexical representation of visual words in the fusiform gyrus , 2002, Neuroreport.
[4] Alfonso Caramazza,et al. VARIETIES OF PURE ALEXIA: THE CASE OF FAILURE TO ACCESS GRAPHEMIC REPRESENTATIONS. , 1998, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[5] K Patterson,et al. Letter-by-letter reading: psychological descriptions of a neurological syndrome. , 1995, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[6] Michal Lavidor,et al. Analysis of standard and non-standard visual word format in the two hemispheres , 2001, Neuropsychologia.
[7] C. Frith,et al. Inattentional blindness versus inattentional amnesia for fixated but ignored words. , 1999, Science.
[8] S. Blumstein,et al. The Role of Segmentation in Phonological Processing: An fMRI Investigation , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[9] M. Albert,et al. Associative visual agnosia without alexia , 1975, Neurology.
[10] Pierre A. Hallé,et al. Where Is the /b/ in “absurde” [apsyrd]? It Is in French Listeners' Minds , 2000 .
[11] S. Greenblatt,et al. Alexia without agraphia or hemianopsia. Anatomical analysis of an autopsied case , 2000 .
[12] Y. Yamane,et al. Complex objects are represented in macaque inferotemporal cortex by the combination of feature columns , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.
[13] Tatjana A. Nazir,et al. Traces of Print Along the Visual Pathway , 2000 .
[14] K. T. Spoehr,et al. The role of orthographic and phonotactic rules in perceiving letter patterns. , 1975, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[15] Denis G. Pelli,et al. The remarkable inefficiency of word recognition , 2003, Nature.
[16] T. Allison,et al. Human extrastriate visual cortex and the perception of faces, words, numbers, and colors. , 1994, Cerebral cortex.
[17] Martha J. Farah,et al. Pure Alexia as a Visual Impairment: A Reconsideration , 1991 .
[18] A. Ishai,et al. Distributed and Overlapping Representations of Faces and Objects in Ventral Temporal Cortex , 2001, Science.
[19] D. Beversdorf,et al. Pure alexia: clinical-pathologic evidence for a lateralized visual language association cortex , 2000 .
[20] N. Logothetis,et al. Shape representation in the inferior temporal cortex of monkeys , 1995, Current Biology.
[21] R. Snider. Brain Mechanisms , 1965, Neurology.
[22] M Coltheart,et al. Basic processes in reading: computation of abstract letter identities. , 1984, Canadian journal of psychology.
[23] S. Dehaene,et al. Language-specific tuning of visual cortex? Functional properties of the Visual Word Form Area. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[24] A. Damasio,et al. The anatomic basis of pure alexia , 1983, Neurology.
[25] S Lehéricy,et al. The visual word form area: spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[26] A. Gomori,et al. Visual agnosia without alexia , 1984, Neurology.
[27] Derek Besner,et al. On the Role of Outline Shape and Word-Specific Visual Pattern in the Identification of Function Words: None , 1989 .
[28] D. Bub,et al. The Neural Substrate for Concrete, Abstract, and Emotional Word Lexica A Positron Emission Tomography Study , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[29] Stéphane Lehéricy,et al. The pathophysiology of letter-by-letter reading , 2004, Neuropsychologia.
[30] R. Salmelin,et al. Dynamics of visual feature analysis and object-level processing in face versus letter-string perception. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[31] Joel R. Meyer,et al. Relation between brain activation and lexical performance , 2003, Human brain mapping.
[32] M. Behrmann,et al. Visual complexity in letter-by-letter reading: Pure alexia is not pure , 1998, Neuropsychologia.
[33] G K Morley,et al. Neurolinguistic analysis of the language abilities of a patient with a "double disconnection syndrome": a case of subangular alexia in the presence of mixed transcortical aphasia. , 1981, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.
[34] J. Fletcher,et al. Brain mechanisms for reading words and pseudowords: an integrated approach. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.
[35] M. Tarr,et al. The Fusiform Face Area is Part of a Network that Processes Faces at the Individual Level , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[36] T. Allison,et al. Electrophysiological studies of human face perception. I: Potentials generated in occipitotemporal cortex by face and non-face stimuli. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.
[37] S E Petersen,et al. The processing of single words studied with positron emission tomography. , 1993, Annual review of neuroscience.
[38] L. Cohen. Number processing in pure alexia: The effect of hemispheric asymmetries and task demands , 1995 .
[39] A. Castro-Caldas,et al. Right hemifield alexia without hemianopia. , 1984, Archives of neurology.
[40] Y. Miyashita,et al. Neural organization for the long-term memory of paired associates , 1991, Nature.
[41] Valerie A. Carr,et al. Spatiotemporal Dynamics of Modality-Specific and Supramodal Word Processing , 2003, Neuron.
[42] T. Poggio,et al. Hierarchical models of object recognition in cortex , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.
[43] N. Kanwisher,et al. The Fusiform Face Area: A Module in Human Extrastriate Cortex Specialized for Face Perception , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[44] B. Weekes. Differential Effects of Number of Letters on Word and Nonword Naming Latency , 1997 .
[45] D. LeBihan,et al. Modulation of Parietal Activation by Semantic Distance in a Number Comparison Task , 2001, NeuroImage.
[46] N. Kanwisher,et al. The Human Body , 2001 .
[47] J. Bowers,et al. Orthographic, phonological, and articulatory contributions to masked letter and word priming. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[48] Andrew W. Ellis,et al. Word Length and Orthographic Neighborhood Size Effects in the Left and Right Cerebral Hemispheres , 2002, Brain and Language.
[49] J. Ziegler,et al. Orthography shapes the perception of speech: The consistency effect in auditory word recognition , 1998 .
[50] T. Allison,et al. Modulation of human extrastriate visual processing by selective attention to colours and words. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[51] D. Le Bihan,et al. Visualizing the Neural Bases of a Disconnection Syndrome with Diffusion Tensor Imaging , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[52] Mark S. Seidenberg,et al. Multiple code activation in word recognition: evidence from rhyme monitoring. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.
[53] A. Dale,et al. Building memories: remembering and forgetting of verbal experiences as predicted by brain activity. , 1998, Science.
[54] T. Allison,et al. Word recognition in the human inferior temporal lobe , 1994, Nature.
[55] P. Skudlarski,et al. Disruption of posterior brain systems for reading in children with developmental dyslexia , 2002, Biological Psychiatry.
[56] S. Dehaene,et al. The priming method: imaging unconscious repetition priming reveals an abstract representation of number in the parietal lobes. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.
[57] S. Petersen,et al. Activation of extrastriate and frontal cortical areas by visual words and word-like stimuli. , 1990, Science.
[58] K. Uutela,et al. Impaired visual word processing in dyslexia revealed with magnetoencephalography , 1996, Annals of neurology.
[59] R. Frackowiak,et al. Demonstrating the implicit processing of visually presented words and pseudowords. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.
[60] Karl J. Friston,et al. A multimodal language region in the ventral visual pathway , 1998, Nature.
[61] James R. Booth,et al. Functional Anatomy of Intra- and Cross-Modal Lexical Tasks , 2002, NeuroImage.
[62] C. Price,et al. The Constraints Functional Neuroimaging Places on Classical Models of Auditory Word Processing , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[63] A. Toga,et al. Mapping sulcal pattern asymmetry and local cortical surface gray matter distribution in vivo: maturation in perisylvian cortices. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.
[64] R. Salmelin,et al. Dynamics of letter string perception in the human occipitotemporal cortex. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[65] M. Farah,et al. Neural Specialization for Letter Recognition , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[66] Elise Temple,et al. Brain mechanisms in normal and dyslexic readers , 2002, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[67] J. L. Nespoulous,et al. Optic Aphasia with Pure Alexia: A Mild Form of Visual Associative Agnosia? A Case Study , 1998, Cortex.
[68] E K Warrington,et al. Prosopagnosia: A Face-Specific Disorder , 1993, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[69] T. Allison,et al. Differential Sensitivity of Human Visual Cortex to Faces, Letterstrings, and Textures: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[70] A. Toga,et al. Three-Dimensional Statistical Analysis of Sulcal Variability in the Human Brain , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[71] T. Allison,et al. Electrophysiological studies of human face perception. III: Effects of top-down processing on face-specific potentials. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.
[72] E. Darcy Burgund,et al. Letter-Case-Specific Priming in the Right Cerebral Hemisphere with a Form-Specific Perceptual Identification Task , 1997, Brain and Cognition.
[73] U. Frith,et al. Explicit and implicit processing of words and pseudowords by adult developmental dyslexics: A search for Wernicke's Wortschatz? , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[74] Keiji Tanaka,et al. Inferotemporal cortex and object vision. , 1996, Annual review of neuroscience.
[75] S. Petersen,et al. Effects of Lexicality, Frequency, and Spelling-to-Sound Consistency on the Functional Anatomy of Reading , 1999, Neuron.
[76] Rafael Malach,et al. Large-Scale Mirror-Symmetry Organization of Human Occipito-Temporal Object Areas , 2003, Neuron.
[77] T. Hendler,et al. A hierarchical axis of object processing stages in the human visual cortex. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.
[78] A Yamadori,et al. Dissociation of letter and picture naming resulting from callosal disconnection , 1998, Neurology.
[79] Joël Pynte,et al. Reading as a Perceptual Process , 2000 .
[80] Thad A Polk,et al. Functional MRI evidence for an abstract, not perceptual, word-form area. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[81] Daniel Holender,et al. Differential Processing of Phonographic and Logographic Single-Digit Numbers by the Two Hemispheres , 2018, Mathematical Disabilities.
[82] J. Dejerine,et al. Contribution a l'etude anatomo-pathologique et clinique des differentes varietes de cecite verbale , 2000 .
[83] Talma Hendler,et al. Eccentricity Bias as an Organizing Principle for Human High-Order Object Areas , 2002, Neuron.
[84] J. Binder,et al. The topography of callosal reading pathways. A case-control analysis. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[85] Joel R. Meyer,et al. Modality independence of word comprehension , 2002, Human brain mapping.
[86] J B Poline,et al. Letter Binding and Invariant Recognition of Masked Words , 2004, Psychological science.
[87] JAMES W. PAPEZ. CEREBRAL MECHANISMS , 1939 .
[88] Samuel H. Greenblatt. Subangular alexia without agraphia or hemianopsia , 2000 .
[89] Joseph T Devlin,et al. The myth of the visual word form area , 2003, NeuroImage.
[90] K R Paap,et al. Word shape's in poor shape for the race to the lexicon. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[91] C. Koch,et al. Category-specific visual responses of single neurons in the human medial temporal lobe , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[92] Antonio R. Damasio,et al. Pure alexia , 1983, Trends in Neurosciences.
[93] Marcus Taft,et al. The influence of orthography on phonological representations in the lexicon , 1985 .
[94] V. Iragui,et al. Alexia without agraphia or hemianopia in parietal infarction. , 1991, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.
[95] G. Humphreys,et al. Disruption to word or letter processing? The origins of case-mixing effects. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[96] S. Edelman,et al. Human Brain Mapping 6:316–328(1998) � A Sequence of Object-Processing Stages Revealed by fMRI in the Human Occipital Lobe , 2022 .
[97] James L. McClelland,et al. An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 2. The contextual enhancement effect and some tests and extensions of the model. , 1982, Psychological review.
[98] T. Nazir,et al. Developing normal reading skills: aspects of the visual processes underlying word recognition. , 2000, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[99] Richard J. Brown. Neuropsychology Mental Structure , 1989 .
[100] Bruce D. McCandliss,et al. The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[101] P. Pietrini,et al. Conjoint and extended neural networks for the computation of speech codes: the neural basis of selective impairment in reading words and pseudowords. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.
[102] M Ingvar,et al. The illiterate brain. Learning to read and write during childhood influences the functional organization of the adult brain. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[103] T Shallice,et al. Word-form dyslexia. , 1980, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[104] Karalyn Patterson,et al. Letter-by-letter Reading: Psychological Descriptions of a Neurologial Syndrome , 1982 .
[105] G. M. Reicher. Perceptual recognition as a function of meaninfulness of stimulus material. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.
[106] F. Kitterle,et al. Visual field effects in the discrimination of sine-wave gratings , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.
[107] Keiji Tanaka,et al. Optical Imaging of Functional Organization in the Monkey Inferotemporal Cortex , 1996, Science.
[108] J B Poline,et al. Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.
[109] M. Behrmann,et al. Impact of learning on representation of parts and wholes in monkey inferotemporal cortex , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.
[110] D. Plaut,et al. A LITERATURE REVIEW AND NEW DATA SUPPORTING AN INTERACTIVE ACCOUNT OF LETTER-BY-LETTER READING. , 1998, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[111] J. Richard Hanley,et al. Reading speed in pure alexia , 1996, Neuropsychologia.
[112] Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al. Object-form topology in the ventral temporal lobe Response to I. Gauthier (2000) , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[113] S. Dehaene,et al. Visual word recognition in the left and right hemispheres: anatomical and functional correlates of peripheral alexias. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.
[114] K. Grill-Spector,et al. fMR-adaptation: a tool for studying the functional properties of human cortical neurons. , 2001, Acta psychologica.