A sensitive period for learning about food

It is proposed here that there is a sensitive period in the first two to three years of life during which humans acquire a basic knowledge of what foods are safe to eat. In support of this, it is shown that willingness to eat a wide variety of foods is greatest between the ages of one and two years, and then declines to low levels by age four. These data also show that children who are introduced to solids unusually late have a narrower diet breadth throughout childhood, perhaps because the duration of the sensitive period has been shortened. By reducing the costs associated with learning, a sensitive period for food learning should be adaptive for any omnivore (including early humans) that remains in the same environment throughout its life.

[1]  J. Mckenzie Social and economic implications of minority food habits , 1967, Proceedings of the Nutrition Society.

[2]  George J. Armelagos,et al.  Consuming Passions: The Anthropology of Eating , 1980 .

[3]  P. Rozin,et al.  The child's conception of food: Differentiation of categories of rejected substances in the 16 months to 5 year age range , 1986, Appetite.

[4]  E. Wilson,et al.  Sociobiology: The New Synthesis , 1975 .

[5]  D. Sherry,et al.  Mother's milk: a medium for transmission of cues reflecting the flavor of mother's diet. , 1973, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[6]  L. Birch Effects of Peer Models' Food Choices and Eating Behaviors on Preschoolers' Food Preferences. , 1980 .

[7]  John Garcia,et al.  Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning , 1966 .

[8]  L. Birch Dimensions of preschool children's food preferences , 1979 .

[9]  L. Birch,et al.  I don't like it; I never tried it: Effects of exposure on two-year-old children's food preferences , 1982, Appetite.

[10]  L. Harper,et al.  The effect of adults' eating on young children's acceptance of unfamiliar foods , 1975 .

[11]  D. Symons Evolution of human sexuality , 1979 .

[12]  M. Crawley Herbivory: the Dynamics of Animal-plant Interactions , 1984 .

[13]  V. Rabinowitch The role of experience in the development and retention of seed preferences in zebra finches. , 1969 .

[14]  A. Fallon,et al.  The Child's Conception of Food: The Development of Categories of Acceptable and Rejected Substances , 1986 .

[15]  A. Logue,et al.  Food preferences in families , 1988, Appetite.

[16]  B. Galef,et al.  Social factors in the poison avoidance and feeding behavior of wild and domesticated rat pups. , 1971, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[17]  Paul Rozin,et al.  The child's conception of food: the development of food rejections with special reference to disgust and contamination sensitivity. , 1984, Child development.

[18]  D. Stephens Change, regularity, and value in the evolution of animal learning , 1991 .

[19]  G. Holton The new synthesis? , 1978 .

[20]  M. Pelchat,et al.  Antecedents and correlates of feeding problems in young children , 1986 .

[21]  P. Rozin Psychobiological and cultural determinants of food choice. , 1976 .

[22]  R. Dirzo,et al.  Herbivores. Their Interaction with Secondary Plant Metabolites. , 1981 .

[23]  B. Rothman,et al.  Only Mothers Know: Patterns of Infant Feeding in Traditional Cultures , 1985 .

[24]  H. Pulliam,et al.  Programmed to Learn: An Essay on the Evolution of Culture , 1980 .

[25]  P. Pliner The Effects of Mere Exposure on Liking for Edible Substances , 1982, Appetite.

[26]  P. Capretta,et al.  Establishment of a flavor preference in rats: importance of nursing and weaning experience. , 1974, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[27]  D. Raphael Breastfeeding and food policy in a hungry world. , 1980 .

[28]  K. Duncker,et al.  Experimental modification of children's food preferences through social suggestion. , 1938 .