The premotor theory of attention as an account for the Simon effect.
暂无分享,去创建一个
Elger L Abrahamse | Elian De Kleine | R. V. D. Van der Lubbe | E. Abrahamse | Rob H J Van der Lubbe | Elian de Kleine
[1] J. R. Simon,et al. Auditory S-R compatibility: the effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. , 1967, The Journal of applied psychology.
[2] R. Wallace,et al. S-R compatibility and the idea of a response code. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.
[3] Edmund Wascher,et al. Dynamic aspects of stimulus-response correspondence: evidence for two mechanisms involved in the Simon effect. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[4] M Eimer,et al. Stimulus-response compatibility and automatic response activation: evidence from psychophysiological studies. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[5] R Verleger,et al. Dimensional overlap between arrows as cueing stimuli and responses?. Evidence from contra-ipsilateral differences in EEG potentials. , 2000, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[6] Carlo Umiltà,et al. Attention shifts produce spatial stimulus codes , 1994, Psychological research.
[7] Robin Walker,et al. Control of voluntary and reflexive saccades , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.
[8] R. Andersen,et al. Intentional maps in posterior parietal cortex. , 2002, Annual review of neuroscience.
[9] U. Ansorge,et al. A Simon effect in memory retrieval: Evidence for the response-discrimination account , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[10] H. Deubel,et al. Selective Dorsal and Ventral Processing: Evidence for a Common Attentional Mechanism in Reaching and Perception , 1998 .
[11] A. H. C. van der Heijden,et al. Selective Attention in Vision , 1991 .
[12] U. Ansorge,et al. Transfer of response codes from choice-response to go/no-go tasks , 2009, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[13] Rob H. J. van der Lubbe,et al. The Involvement of Occipital Cortex in the Early Blind in Auditory and Tactile Duration Discrimination Tasks , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[14] R. Verleger,et al. Mechanisms underlying spatial coding in a multiple-item Simon task , 2005, Psychological research.
[15] Rob H. J. van der Lubbe,et al. Interruption from irrelevant auditory and visual onsets even when attention is in a focused state , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.
[16] Edmund Wascher,et al. Response coding in the Simon task , 2007, Psychological research.
[17] R. Klein,et al. Does Oculomotor Readiness Mediate Cognitive Control of Visual-Attention - Revisited , 1994 .
[18] J. C. Woestenburg,et al. The influence of peripheral precues on the tendency to react towards a lateral relevant stimulus with multiple-item arrays , 1999, Biological Psychology.
[19] E. Soetens,et al. Control over location-based response activation in the Simon task: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[20] R. V. D. Van der Lubbe,et al. Endogenous orienting modulates the Simon effect: critical factors in experimental design , 2007, Psychological research.
[21] B. Hommel,et al. A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task , 2004, Psychological research.
[22] B. Hommel. Inverting the Simon effect by intention , 1993 .
[23] U. Ansorge,et al. A response-discrimination account of the Simon effect. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[24] C. Umilta,et al. Simon effect with and without awareness of the accessory stimulus. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[25] A. H. C. van der Heijden. Attention in Vision: Perception, Communication and Action , 2004 .
[26] Wim Notebaert,et al. Sequential analysis of a Simon task – evidence for an attention-shift account , 2001, Psychological research.
[27] J. R. Simon. The Effects of an Irrelevant Directional CUE on Human Information Processing , 1990 .
[28] J. R. Simon,et al. Processing auditory information: interaction of two population stereotypes. , 1976, The Journal of applied psychology.
[29] T. Stoffer,et al. The functional role of attention for spatial coding in the Simon effect , 1994, Psychological research.
[30] Elger L Abrahamse,et al. The premotor theory of attention and the Simon effect. , 2011, Acta psychologica.
[31] R. Verleger,et al. Spatiotemporal overlap between brain activation related to saccade preparation and attentional orienting , 2006, Brain Research.
[32] S. Brandt,et al. Executive control over response priming and conflict: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.
[33] Martin Eimer,et al. Covert manual response preparation triggers attentional shifts: ERP evidence for the premotor theory of attention , 2005, Neuropsychologia.
[34] M. Corbetta,et al. Functional Organization of Human Intraparietal and Frontal Cortex for Attending, Looking, and Pointing , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[35] C Iani,et al. The Simon effect occurs relative to the direction of an attention shift. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[36] A. Postma,et al. Spatial attention triggered by unimodal, crossmodal, and bimodal exogenous cues: a comparison of reflexive orienting mechanisms , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[37] Robert W. Proctor,et al. Stimulus-Response Compatibility: An Integrated Perspective , 1990 .
[38] A. Kingstone,et al. Topic: Cognition , 2003 .
[39] L. Zimba,et al. Attention precuing and Simon effects: A test of the attention-coding account of the Simon effect , 1995 .
[40] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention , 1987, Neuropsychologia.
[41] B. Hommel. Spontaneous decay of response-code activation , 1994, Psychological research.
[42] Bernhard Hommel,et al. Responding to object files: Automatic integration of spatial information revealed by stimulus-response compatibility effects , 2002, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[43] R. Klein,et al. The components of visual attention and the ubiquitous Simon effect. , 2011, Acta psychologica.
[44] B. Hommel. Inverting the Simon effect intention: Determinants of direction and extent of effects of irrelevant spatial information. , 1993 .
[45] John Christie,et al. On the uniqueness of attentional capture by uninformative gaze cues: facilitation interacts with the Simon effect and is rarely followed by IOR. , 2007, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.
[46] Marijn E. Struiksma,et al. What is the link between language and spatial images? Behavioral and neural findings in blind and sighted individuals. , 2009, Acta psychologica.
[47] R. Verleger,et al. Validity and boundary conditions of automatic response activation in the Simon task. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[48] H. Deubel,et al. Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism , 1996, Vision Research.
[49] R. Lubbe,et al. FAILURE OF THE EXTENDED CONTINGENT ATTENTIONAL CAPTURE ACCOUNT IN MULTIMODAL SETTINGS , 2006 .
[50] M. Eimer. The N2pc component as an indicator of attentional selectivity. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[51] J. Driver,et al. Audiovisual links in exogenous covert spatial orienting , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.
[52] B. Hommel. The Quarterly Journal Of Experimental Psychology, 1996 , 49a (3) , 546 571 , 2022 .
[53] J. R. Simon,et al. Reactions toward the source of stimulation. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.
[54] K. Heilman,et al. Attentional factors in the occurrence of stimulus-response compatibility effects , 1988, Neuropsychologia.
[55] Rolf Verleger,et al. Influence of Time Pressure in a Simple Response Task, a Choice- by-Location Task, and the Simon Task , 2001 .
[56] T. Stoffer. Attentional focussing and spatial stimulus-response compatibility , 1991, Psychological research.