It is all me: the effect of viewpoint on visual–vestibular recalibration

Participants performed a visual–vestibular motor recalibration task in virtual reality. The task consisted of keeping the extended arm and hand stable in space during a whole-body rotation induced by a robotic wheelchair. Performance was first quantified in a pre-test in which no visual feedback was available during the rotation. During the subsequent adaptation phase, optical flow resulting from body rotation was provided. This visual feedback was manipulated to create the illusion of a smaller rotational movement than actually occurred, hereby altering the visual–vestibular mapping. The effects of the adaptation phase on hand stabilization performance were measured during a post-test that was identical to the pre-test. Three different groups of subjects were exposed to different perspectives on the visual scene, i.e., first-person, top view, or mirror view. Sensorimotor adaptation occurred for all three viewpoint conditions, performance in the post-test session showing a marked under-compensation relative to the pre-test performance. In other words, all viewpoints gave rise to a remapping between vestibular input and the motor output required to stabilize the arm. Furthermore, the first-person and mirror view adaptation induced a significant decrease in variability of the stabilization performance. Such variability reduction was not observed for the top view adaptation. These results suggest that even if all three viewpoints can evoke substantial adaptation aftereffects, the more naturalistic first-person view and the richer mirror view should be preferred when reducing motor variability constitutes an important issue.

[1]  Gordon M. Redding,et al.  Applications of prism adaptation: a tutorial in theory and method , 2005, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[2]  Daniel Thalmann,et al.  The benefits of third-person perspective in virtual and augmented reality? , 2006, VRST '06.

[3]  Jean-Louis Vercher,et al.  On the nature of the vestibular control of arm-reaching movements during whole-body rotations , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  P. Verschure,et al.  New Technologies and Concepts for Rehabilitation in the Acute Phase of Stroke: A Collaborative Matrix , 2007, Neurodegenerative Diseases.

[5]  R. Chan,et al.  Preconditioners for non‐Hermitian Toeplitz systems , 2001 .

[6]  E Bizzi,et al.  Augmented Feedback Presented in a Virtual Environment Accelerates Learning of a Difficult Motor Task. , 1997, Journal of motor behavior.

[7]  G. Kwakkel,et al.  Probability of regaining dexterity in the flaccid upper limb: impact of severity of paresis and time since onset in acute stroke. , 2003, Stroke.

[8]  Michel Guerraz,et al.  Insights into the control of arm movement during body motion as revealed by EMG analyses , 2010, Brain Research.

[9]  Daniel Thalmann,et al.  Providing the best Third-Person Perspective to a Video-Through HMD , 2010 .

[10]  Etienne Guillaud,et al.  Controlling reaching movements during self-motion: body-fixed versus Earth-fixed targets. , 2006, Motor control.

[11]  Konrad Paul Körding,et al.  The loss function of sensorimotor learning. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[12]  Jeffery J. Summers,et al.  Bilateral and unilateral movement training on upper limb function in chronic stroke patients: A TMS study , 2007, Journal of the Neurological Sciences.

[13]  J. Loomis,et al.  Immersive virtual environment technology as a basic research tool in psychology , 1999, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[14]  Marjolein P. M. Kammers,et al.  The rubber hand illusion in action , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  W. Becker,et al.  Vestibular, optokinetic, and cognitive contribution to the guidance of passive self-rotation toward instructed targets , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  L Tremblay,et al.  Specificity of practice: the case of powerlifting. , 1998, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[17]  David B. Kaber,et al.  The Effect of Automated Compensation for Incongruent Axes on Teleoperator Performance , 1998, Hum. Factors.

[18]  Peter Willemsen,et al.  The Influence of Restricted Viewing Conditions on Egocentric Distance Perception: Implications for Real and Virtual Indoor Environments , 2005, Perception.

[19]  H. Pick,et al.  Gaze-contingent prism adaptation: optical and motor factors. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[20]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Estimation of passive horizontal linear whole-body displacement in humans. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  Holger Regenbrecht,et al.  The Experience of Presence: Factor Analytic Insights , 2001, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[22]  Taku Komura,et al.  Computing inverse kinematics with linear programming , 2005, VRST '05.

[23]  R B Welch,et al.  An examination of the relationship between visual capture and prism adaptation , 1979, Perception & psychophysics.

[24]  Michael J. Singer,et al.  Measuring Presence in Virtual Environments: A Presence Questionnaire , 1998, Presence.

[25]  Christophe Bourdin,et al.  On-line versus off-line vestibular-evoked control of goal-directed arm movements , 2002, Neuroreport.

[26]  M. Merzenich,et al.  Brain plasticity and functional losses in the aged: scientific bases for a novel intervention. , 2006, Progress in brain research.

[27]  L. Harris,et al.  Visual and non-visual cues in the perception of linear self motion , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  T. Olsen,et al.  Arm and leg paresis as outcome predictors in stroke rehabilitation. , 1990, Stroke.

[29]  E. Tunik,et al.  Sensorimotor training in virtual reality: a review. , 2009, NeuroRehabilitation.

[30]  L Proteau,et al.  A Sensorimotor Basis for Motor Learning: Evidence Indicating Specificity of Practice , 1992, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[31]  Digby Elliott,et al.  Vision and Goal-Directed Movement: Neurobehavioral Perspectives , 2010 .

[32]  Melanie Chan Embodiment, Perception, and Virtual Reality , 2001, Cognitive Technology.

[33]  G. Kwakkel,et al.  Intensity of leg and arm training after primary middle-cerebral-artery stroke: a randomised trial , 1999, The Lancet.

[34]  L. S. Tamkei,et al.  Effect of viewing angle on arm reaching while standing in a virtual environment: potential for virtual rehabilitation. , 2010, Acta psychologica.

[35]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see , 1998, Nature.

[36]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Spatial orientation in humans: perception of angular whole-body displacements in two-dimensional trajectories , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[37]  I. Israël,et al.  Self-rotation estimate about the vertical axis. , 1995, Acta oto-laryngologica.

[38]  Jack M. Loomis,et al.  Visual perception of egocentric distance in real and virtual environments. , 2003 .

[39]  M. Tarr,et al.  Virtual reality in behavioral neuroscience and beyond , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[40]  S. Masiero,et al.  Robotic-assisted rehabilitation of the upper limb after acute stroke. , 2007, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[41]  T Platz,et al.  Impairment-oriented training or Bobath therapy for severe arm paresis after stroke: a single-blind, multicentre randomized controlled trial , 2005, Clinical rehabilitation.

[42]  H. Ring,et al.  Controlled study of neuroprosthetic functional electrical stimulation in sub-acute post-stroke rehabilitation. , 2005, Journal of rehabilitation medicine.

[43]  M. Levin,et al.  Virtual Reality in Stroke Rehabilitation: A Systematic Review of its Effectiveness for Upper Limb Motor Recovery , 2007, Topics in stroke rehabilitation.