Global Visual Motion Sensitivity: Associations with Parietal Area and Children's Mathematical Cognition

Sensitivity to global visual motion has been proposed as a signature of brain development, related to the dorsal rather than ventral cortical stream. Thresholds for global motion have been found to be elevated more than for global static form in many developmental disorders, leading to the idea of “dorsal stream vulnerability.” Here we explore the association of global motion thresholds with individual differences in children's brain development, in a group of typically developing 5- to 12-year-olds. Good performance was associated with a relative increase in parietal lobe surface area, most strongly around the intraparietal sulcus and decrease in occipital area. In line with the involvement of intraparietal sulcus, areas in visuospatial and numerical cognition, we also found that global motion performance was correlated with tests of visuomotor integration and numerical skills. Individual differences in global form detection showed none of these anatomical or cognitive correlations. This suggests that the correlations with motion sensitivity are unlikely to reflect general perceptual or attentional abilities required for both form and motion. We conclude that individual developmental variations in global motion processing are not linked to greater area in the extrastriate visual areas, which initially process such motion, but in the parietal systems that make decisions based on this information. The overlap with visuospatial and numerical abilities may indicate the anatomical substrate of the “dorsal stream vulnerability” proposed as characterizing neurodevelopmental disorders.

[1]  D. V. van Essen,et al.  Selectivity for polar, hyperbolic, and Cartesian gratings in macaque visual cortex. , 1993, Science.

[2]  David C Burr,et al.  Visual sustained attention and numerosity sensitivity correlate with math achievement in children. , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[3]  Andreas Nieder,et al.  Neuronal population coding of continuous and discrete quantity in the primate posterior parietal cortex , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[4]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Representation of number in the brain. , 2009, Annual review of neuroscience.

[5]  Kenneth F. Valyear,et al.  Human parietal cortex in action , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[6]  D G Gadian,et al.  Calculation difficulties in children of very low birthweight: a neural correlate. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[7]  Armin Raznahan,et al.  How Does Your Cortex Grow? , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  A. Dale,et al.  Hierarchical Genetic Organization of Human Cortical Surface Area , 2012, Science.

[9]  M. Goodale,et al.  The visual brain in action , 1995 .

[10]  Janette Atkinson,et al.  From genes to brain development to phenotypic behavior: "dorsal-stream vulnerability" in relation to spatial cognition, attention, and planning of actions in Williams syndrome (WS) and other developmental disorders. , 2011, Progress in brain research.

[11]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Developmental trajectories of grey and white matter in dyscalculia , 2013, Trends in Neuroscience and Education.

[12]  M Boucart,et al.  Neural correlates of implicit object identification , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  C. Galletti,et al.  Human V6: The Medial Motion Area , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[14]  Masud Husain,et al.  Visuomotor functions of the posterior parietal cortex , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  K. Kucian,et al.  Impaired neural networks for approximate calculation in dyscalculic children: a functional MRI study , 2006, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[16]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Discrete and analogue quantity processing in the parietal lobe: a functional MRI study. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[17]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Neural basis of mathematical cognition , 2011, Current Biology.

[18]  Gavin R. Price,et al.  Impaired parietal magnitude processing in developmental dyscalculia , 2007, Current Biology.

[19]  Pol Ghesquière,et al.  Coherent motion sensitivity predicts individual differences in subtraction. , 2011, Research in developmental disabilities.

[20]  Tal Kenet,et al.  The Pediatric Imaging, Neurocognition, and Genetics (PING) Data Repository , 2016, NeuroImage.

[21]  Nikos Makris,et al.  Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[22]  Mark E. Wheeler,et al.  Content-specific evidence accumulation in inferior temporal cortex during perceptual decision-making , 2015, NeuroImage.

[23]  Thomas Haarmeier,et al.  Processing of Coherent Visual Motion in Topographically Organized Visual Areas in Human Cerebral Cortex , 2012, Brain Topography.

[24]  Maria Concetta Morrone,et al.  BOLD Response Selective to Flow-Motion in Very Young Infants , 2015, PLoS biology.

[25]  Vijay K. Venkatraman,et al.  Multimodal imaging of the self-regulating developing brain , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[26]  Anders M. Dale,et al.  A web-portal for interactive data exploration, visualization, and hypothesis testing , 2014, Front. Neuroinform..

[27]  J. Atkinson,et al.  Visual attention in the first years: typical development and developmental disorders , 2012, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[28]  Dwight J. Kravitz,et al.  A new neural framework for visuospatial processing , 2011, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[29]  W. Newsome,et al.  Neural basis of a perceptual decision in the parietal cortex (area LIP) of the rhesus monkey. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  Timothy D. Hanks,et al.  Microstimulation of macaque area LIP affects decision-making in a motion discrimination task , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Distinct Cerebral Pathways for Object Identity and Number in Human Infants , 2008, PLoS biology.

[32]  O. Braddick,et al.  Brain Areas Sensitive to Coherent Visual Motion , 2001, Perception.

[33]  Vijay K. Venkatraman,et al.  Neuroanatomical Assessment of Biological Maturity , 2012, Current Biology.

[34]  Janette Atkinson,et al.  Visual and visuocognitive development in children born very prematurely. , 2007, Progress in brain research.

[35]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  A general mechanism for perceptual decision-making in the human brain , 2004, Nature.

[36]  J. Atkinson,et al.  Development of human visual function , 2011, Vision Research.

[37]  Philippe Pinel,et al.  Tuning Curves for Approximate Numerosity in the Human Intraparietal Sulcus , 2004, Neuron.

[38]  T D Albright,et al.  Visual motion perception. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[39]  Anders M. Dale,et al.  Anxiety is related to indices of cortical maturation in typically developing children and adolescents , 2016, Brain Structure and Function.

[40]  C. Tyler,et al.  Bayesian adaptive estimation of psychometric slope and threshold , 1999, Vision Research.

[41]  Demis Basso,et al.  Motion on Numbers: Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation on the Ventral Intraparietal Sulcus Alters Both Numerical and Motion Processes , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[42]  Guy A. Orban,et al.  Similarities and differences in motion processing between the human and macaque brain: evidence from fMRI , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  Marie-Pascale Noël,et al.  Neural Correlates of Symbolic Number Comparison in Developmental Dyscalculia , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[44]  H. Sigmundsson,et al.  Are poor mathematics skills associated with visual deficits in temporal processing? , 2010, Neuroscience Letters.

[45]  J. Atkinson,et al.  Reorganization of Global Form and Motion Processing during Human Visual Development , 2010, Current Biology.

[46]  Vinod Menon,et al.  Developmental cognitive neuroscience of arithmetic: implications for learning and education , 2010, ZDM : the international journal on mathematics education.

[47]  Vincenzo Bonifati,et al.  Assessment of neurological and behavioural function: the NIH Toolbox , 2010 .

[48]  John Wattam-Bell,et al.  Coherence thresholds for discrimination of motion direction in infants , 1994, Vision Research.

[49]  T. Brown,et al.  Brain Development During the Preschool Years , 2012, Neuropsychology Review.

[50]  J. Gilmore,et al.  Dynamic Development of Regional Cortical Thickness and Surface Area in Early Childhood. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[51]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Developmental Specialization in the Right Intraparietal Sulcus for the Abstract Representation of Numerical Magnitude , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[52]  A. Dale,et al.  Cortical Surface-Based Analysis II: Inflation, Flattening, and a Surface-Based Coordinate System , 1999, NeuroImage.

[53]  S. Dehaene,et al.  THREE PARIETAL CIRCUITS FOR NUMBER PROCESSING , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[54]  Jane Holmes Bernstein,et al.  Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration , 2010, Encyclopedia of Autism Spectrum Disorders.

[55]  J. F. Stein,et al.  Magnocellular visual function and children's single word reading , 1998, Vision Research.

[56]  G. Orban,et al.  Motion-responsive regions of the human brain , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[57]  E. J. Carter,et al.  Functional Imaging of Numerical Processing in Adults and 4-y-Old Children , 2006, PLoS biology.

[58]  Anders M. Dale,et al.  Toward an integrative science of the developing human mind and brain: Focus on the developing cortex , 2015, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[59]  Anders M. Dale,et al.  Cortical Surface-Based Analysis I. Segmentation and Surface Reconstruction , 1999, NeuroImage.

[60]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Object vision and spatial vision: two cortical pathways , 1983, Trends in Neurosciences.

[61]  A. Lundervold,et al.  High-expanding cortical regions in human development and evolution are related to higher intellectual abilities. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[62]  Linda Chang,et al.  The NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery: results from a large normative developmental sample (PING). , 2014, Neuropsychology.

[63]  Janette Atkinson,et al.  Dorsal and ventral stream sensitivity in normal development and hemiplegia , 2002, Neuroreport.

[64]  Mariagrazia Benassi,et al.  Coherence motion perception in developmental dyslexia: a meta-analysis of behavioral studies. , 2010, Dyslexia.

[65]  Stine K. Krogsrud,et al.  Development and aging of cortical thickness correspond to genetic organization patterns , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[66]  W. Newsome,et al.  A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT) , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[67]  J. Movshon,et al.  The analysis of visual motion: a comparison of neuronal and psychophysical performance , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[68]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Developmental change in the acuity of the "Number Sense": The Approximate Number System in 3-, 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds and adults. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[69]  Anders M. Dale,et al.  Sequence-independent segmentation of magnetic resonance images , 2004, NeuroImage.

[70]  J. Atkinson,et al.  Normal and anomalous development of visual motion processing: motion coherence and ‘dorsal-stream vulnerability’ , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[71]  R. Turner,et al.  Form and motion coherence activate independent, but not dorsal/ventral segregated, networks in the human brain , 2000, Current Biology.

[72]  Janette Atkinson,et al.  Dorsal stream vulnerability and autistic disorders: the importance of comparative studies of form and motion coherence in typically developing children and children with developmental disorders , 2005 .

[73]  Matthew Inglis,et al.  Indexing the approximate number system. , 2014, Acta psychologica.

[74]  A. Rees,et al.  Sensitivity to dynamic auditory and visual stimuli predicts nonword reading ability in both dyslexic and normal readers , 1998, Current Biology.