Adaptation to astigmatic blur.

Adapting to blurred or sharpened images alters the perceived focus of subsequently viewed images. We examined whether these adaptation effects could arise from actual sphero-cylindrical refractive errors, by testing aftereffects in images simulating second-order astigmatism. Image blur was varied from negative (vertical) through isotropic to positive (horizontal) astigmatism while maintaining constant blur strength. A 2AFC staircase was used to estimate the stimulus that appeared isotropically blurred before or after adapting to images with astigmatism. Adaptation to horizontal blur caused isotropically blurred images to appear vertically biased and vice versa, shifting the perceived isotropic point toward the adapting level. Aftereffects were similar for different types of images and showed partial selectivity so that strongest effects generally occurred when testing and adapting images were the same. Further experiments explored whether the adaptation depended more strongly on the blurring or "fuzziness" in the images vs. the apparent "figural" changes introduced by the blur, by comparing how the aftereffects transfer across changes in size or orientation. Our results suggest that strong selective adaptation can occur for different lower order aberrations of the eye and that these may be at least partly driven by the apparent figural changes that blurring introduces into the retinal image.

[1]  David R Williams,et al.  Neural compensation for the best aberration correction. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[2]  S. Barbero,et al.  Optical response to LASIK surgery for myopia from total and corneal aberration measurements. , 2001, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[3]  D. Williams,et al.  Monochromatic aberrations of the human eye in a large population. , 2001, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[4]  H. Wallach,et al.  Figural aftereffects; an investigation of visual processes. , 1944 .

[5]  Wendy J. Adams,et al.  Adaptation to three-dimensional distortions in human vision , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[6]  Geunyoung Yoon,et al.  Neural compensation for long-term asymmetric optical blur to improve visual performance in keratoconic eyes. , 2010, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[7]  Stuart Anstis,et al.  Was El Greco Astigmatic? , 2002, Leonardo.

[8]  D R Williams,et al.  Supernormal vision and high-resolution retinal imaging through adaptive optics. , 1997, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[9]  Mark S. Rea,et al.  Visual Performance with Realistic Methods of Changing Contrast , 1981 .

[10]  Geunyoung Yoon,et al.  Visual performance after correcting higher order aberrations in keratoconic eyes. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[11]  James R Tresilian,et al.  Improving vision: neural compensation for optical defocus , 1998, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[12]  M. Georgeson,et al.  Contrast constancy: deblurring in human vision by spatial frequency channels. , 1975, The Journal of physiology.

[13]  Yoko Mizokami,et al.  Neural adjustments to chromatic blur. , 2006, Spatial vision.

[14]  Richard Held,et al.  Increased accommodation following adaptation to image blur in myopes. , 2004, Journal of Vision.

[15]  Michael A. Webster,et al.  Adaptation to blur , 2001, IS&T/SPIE Electronic Imaging.

[16]  C. Clifford,et al.  Pulling Faces: An Investigation of the Face-Distortion Aftereffect , 2003, Perception.

[17]  Eric Papas,et al.  Performance Standards for Toric Soft Contact Lenses , 2007, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[18]  Susana Marcos,et al.  Visual performance with real-life tasks under adaptive-optics ocular aberration correction. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[19]  A. Bradley,et al.  Statistical variation of aberration structure and image quality in a normal population of healthy eyes. , 2002, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[20]  Aaron C Bilson,et al.  Visual adjustments to temporal blur. , 2005, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[21]  Susana Marcos,et al.  Adapting to blur produced by ocular high-order aberrations. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[22]  Michael A. Webster,et al.  Neural adjustments to image blur , 2010 .

[23]  D L Guyton,et al.  Prescribing cylinders: the problem of distortion. , 1977, Survey of ophthalmology.

[24]  W Neil Charman,et al.  Hartmann-Shack technique and refraction across the horizontal visual field. , 2003, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[25]  Andrew Metha,et al.  Enhanced contrast sensitivity confirms active compensation in blur adaptation. , 2010, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[26]  T W Raasch,et al.  Spherocylindrical Refractive Errors and Visual Acuity , 1995, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[27]  L. Thibos,et al.  Power Vectors: An Application of Fourier Analysis to the Description and Statistical Analysis of Refractive Error , 1997, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[28]  Susana Marcos,et al.  Influence of adaptive-optics ocular aberration correction on visual acuity at different luminances and contrast polarities. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[29]  John Edgar,et al.  Performance Standards , 1979, Journal of MultiDisciplinary Evaluation.

[30]  K. Pesudovs,et al.  Decreased Uncorrected Vision After a Period of Distance Fixation with Spectacle Wear , 1993, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[31]  Otto H. MacLin,et al.  Figural aftereffects in the perception of faces , 1999, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[32]  David A Atchison,et al.  Refraction and Aberration Across the Horizontal Central 10° of the Visual Field , 2006, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[33]  E. Peli,et al.  Shape and individual variability of the blur adaptation curve , 2010, Vision Research.

[34]  Keith A May,et al.  From filters to features: scale-space analysis of edge and blur coding in human vision. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[35]  Arthur Bradley,et al.  Orientation and spatial frequency selectivity of adaptation to color and luminance gratings , 1988, Vision Research.

[36]  M. Webster,et al.  Contrast adaptation and the spatial structure of natural images. , 1997, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[37]  David R Williams,et al.  Neural compensation for the eye's optical aberrations. , 2004, Journal of vision.

[38]  Mark Rosenfield,et al.  Blur Adaptation in Myopes , 2004, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[39]  D. Field,et al.  Visual sensitivity, blur and the sources of variability in the amplitude spectra of natural scenes , 1997, Vision Research.

[40]  C. M. Cicerone,et al.  The relative numbers of long-wavelength-sensitive to middle-wavelength-sensitive cones in the human fovea centralis , 1989, Vision Research.

[41]  Susana Marcos,et al.  Accommodative lag and fluctuations when optical aberrations are manipulated. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[42]  C. Chubb,et al.  The size-tuning of the face-distortion after-effect , 2001, Vision Research.

[43]  P. Artal,et al.  The Effect of Correcting Small Astigmatisms on Visual Acuity , 2006 .

[44]  Norberto López-Gil,et al.  Ocular wave-front aberration statistics in a normal young population , 2002, Vision Research.

[45]  Michael Belkin,et al.  Adaptation to astigmatic lens: Effects on lateral interactions , 2010 .

[46]  Robert A. Jacobs,et al.  Depth-dependent blur adaptation , 2004, Vision Research.

[47]  C Blakemore,et al.  On the existence of neurones in the human visual system selectively sensitive to the orientation and size of retinal images , 1969, The Journal of physiology.