Spatial and temporal dynamics of presaccadic attentional facilitation before pro- and antisaccades.

The premotor theory of attention and the visual attention model make different predictions about the temporal and spatial allocation of presaccadic attentional facilitation. The current experiment investigated the spatial and temporal dynamics of presaccadic attentional facilitation during pro- and antisaccade planning; we investigated whether attention shifts only to the saccade goal location or to the target location or elsewhere, and when. Participants performed a dual-task paradigm with blocks of either anti- or prosaccades and also discriminated symbols appearing at different locations before saccade onset (measure of attentional allocation). In prosaccades blocks, correct prosaccade discrimination was best at the target location, while during errors, discrimination was best at the location opposite to the target location. This pattern was inversed in antisaccades blocks, although discrimination remained high opposite to the target location. In addition, we took the benefit of a large range of saccadic landing positions and showed that performance across both types of saccades was best at the actual saccade goal location (where the eye will actually land) rather than at the instructed position. Finally, temporal analyses showed that discrimination remained highest at the saccade goal location, from long before to closer to saccade onset, increasing slightly for antisaccades closer to saccade onset. These findings are in line with the premises of the premotor theory of attention, showing that attentional allocation is primarily linked both temporally and spatially to the saccade goal location.

[1]  Robert M. McPeek,et al.  Concurrent processing of saccades in visual search , 2000, Vision Research.

[2]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[3]  Denis Pélisson,et al.  Plastic Modification of Anti-Saccades: Adaptation of Saccadic Eye Movements Aimed at a Virtual Target , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  M. Carrasco,et al.  Rapid Simultaneous Enhancement of Visual Sensitivity and Perceived Contrast during Saccade Preparation , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  H. Deubel,et al.  Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism , 1996, Vision Research.

[6]  A Straube,et al.  Relation between the metrics of the presaccadic attention shift and of the saccade before and after saccadic adaptation. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Orienting of attention and eye movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  Heiner Deubel,et al.  Attention allocation before antisaccades. , 2016, Journal of vision.

[9]  D. Robinson,et al.  Shared neural control of attentional shifts and eye movements , 1996, Nature.

[10]  Pierre Pouget,et al.  Attentional selection during preparation of eye movements , 2004, Psychological research.

[11]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions , 1978, Vision Research.

[12]  D. Gitelman,et al.  Covert Visual Spatial Orienting and Saccades: Overlapping Neural Systems , 2000, NeuroImage.

[13]  R. Remington,et al.  Eye Movement Targets Are Released from Visual Crowding , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  Eric Castet,et al.  Spatiotemporal dynamics of visual attention during saccade preparation: Independence and coupling between attention and movement planning. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[15]  R. Carpenter,et al.  Countermanding saccades in humans , 1999, Vision Research.

[16]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A Common Network of Functional Areas for Attention and Eye Movements , 1998, Neuron.

[17]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Predictive remapping of attention across eye movements , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[18]  T Moore,et al.  Control of eye movements and spatial attention. , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[19]  J. Hoffman,et al.  The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[20]  H. Deubel The time course of presaccadic attention shifts , 2008, Psychological research.

[21]  R. McPeek,et al.  The eye dominates in guiding attention during simultaneous eye and hand movements. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[22]  B. Dosher,et al.  The role of attention in the programming of saccades , 1995, Vision Research.

[23]  W. Schneider VAM: A neuro-cognitive model for visual attention control of segmentation, object recognition, and space-based motor action , 1995 .

[24]  D. Munoz,et al.  Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[25]  Thérèse Collins,et al.  Eye movement signals influence perception: Evidence from the adaptation of reactive and volitional saccades , 2006, Vision Research.

[26]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Parallel allocation of attention prior to the execution of saccade sequences. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  J. Theeuwes Exogenous and endogenous control of attention: The effect of visual onsets and offsets , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[28]  Ari Z. Zivotofsky,et al.  Back to basics: The effects of block vs. interleaved trial administration on pro- and anti-saccade performance , 2017, PloS one.

[29]  M. Schlag-Rey,et al.  Primate antisaccades. I. Behavioral characteristics. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  D. Munoz,et al.  Age-related trends in saccade characteristics among the elderly , 2011, Neurobiology of Aging.

[31]  T. Moore,et al.  Microstimulation of the frontal eye field and its effects on covert spatial attention. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  Josh Wallman,et al.  Gain adaptation of exogenous shifts of visual attention , 2002, Vision Research.

[33]  H. Pashler,et al.  Evidence for split attentional foci. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[34]  D. Munoz,et al.  Saccade execution suppresses discrimination at distractor locations rather than enhancing the saccade goal location , 2015, The European journal of neuroscience.

[35]  Cristina Massen,et al.  Parallel programming of exogenous and endogenous components in the antisaccade task , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[36]  Eric Castet,et al.  Dynamics of attentional deployment during saccadic programming. , 2006, Journal of vision.

[37]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Space and selective attention , 1994 .

[38]  Robert M McPeek,et al.  Attentional Cueing at the Saccade Goal, Not at the Target Location, Facilitates Saccades , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  Michael S. Beauchamp,et al.  A Parametric fMRI Study of Overt and Covert Shifts of Visuospatial Attention , 2001, NeuroImage.

[40]  Lauren E. Ethridge,et al.  Consider the context: blocked versus interleaved presentation of antisaccade trials. , 2009, Psychophysiology.

[41]  Thérèse Collins,et al.  Saccadic adaptation shifts the pre-saccadic attention focus , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[42]  Erik Blaser,et al.  The accuracy and precision of saccades to small and large targets , 1995, Vision Research.

[43]  D. Pélisson,et al.  Sensorimotor adaptation of saccadic eye movements , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[44]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[45]  Thérèse Collins,et al.  Visual versus motor vector inversions in the antisaccade task: a behavioral investigation with saccadic adaptation. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  J. Schall,et al.  Countermanding saccades in macaque , 1995, Visual Neuroscience.