Common and Specific Contributions of the Intraparietal Sulci to Numerosity and Length Processing

Numerical and spatial magnitude processing have long been intimately associated, leading to the suggestion that they share a common system of magnitude representation. Although separate investigations on the cerebral areas involved in numerosity and spatial estimation point toward the parietal cortex, the precise anatomical overlap, if any, has not yet been directly established. Here, functional magnetic resonance imaging was used to localize the cerebral network involved in processing both numerosity and length. Blood oxygenation level‐dependent signal changes were measured while healthy volunteers were making numerosity comparisons on linear arrays of dots, and length comparisons on discrete linear arrays of dots and continuous rectangles. The results show the bilateral involvement of parietal regions around the intraparietal sulci in explicit and implicit processing of numerosity, and a right lateralized occipitoparietal network activation in length processing; numerosity and length processing both activate the right IPS and the precentral gyrus. By excluding the mandatory intrinsic spatial processing of arrays, we demonstrate that the left IPS is involved in numerosity processing only, whereas the right IPS underlies a common processing mechanism or representation of spatial and numerical magnitude. Hum Brain Mapp 2009. © 2009 Wiley‐Liss, Inc.

[1]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Single-trial classification of parallel pre-attentive and serial attentive processes using functional magnetic resonance imaging , 2003, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[2]  A. Milner,et al.  Distortion of size perception in visuospatial neglect , 1995, Current Biology.

[3]  G. Orban,et al.  Parietal Representation of Symbolic and Nonsymbolic Magnitude , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[4]  Timing ability and numerical competence in rats. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[5]  D. Boisson,et al.  Does Action Make the Link Between Number and Space Representation? , 2004, Psychological science.

[6]  Jesper Andersson,et al.  Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic , 2005, NeuroImage.

[7]  O. Houdé Numerical development: From the infant to the child. Wynn's (1992) paradigm in 2- and 3-year olds☆ , 1997 .

[8]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Differential Contributions of the Left and Right Inferior Parietal Lobules to Number Processing , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[9]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Exact and Approximate Judgements of Visual and Auditory Numerosity: an Fmri Study , 2006 .

[10]  Stefan Golaszewski,et al.  Neural correlates of distance and congruity effects in a numerical Stroop task: an event-related fMRI study , 2005, NeuroImage.

[11]  Joel R. Meyer,et al.  A large-scale distributed network for covert spatial attention: further anatomical delineation based on stringent behavioural and cognitive controls. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[12]  B. Mazoyer,et al.  Cognitive inhibition of number/length interference in a Piaget‐like task in young adults: Evidence from ERPs and fMRI , 2006, Human brain mapping.

[13]  Filippo Brighina,et al.  Contralateral neglect induced by right posterior parietal rTMS in healthy subjects , 2000, Neuroreport.

[14]  Gereon R. Fink,et al.  Spatial Cognition: Where We Were and Where We Are , 2001, NeuroImage.

[15]  A. Nobre,et al.  Where and When to Pay Attention: The Neural Systems for Directing Attention to Spatial Locations and to Time Intervals as Revealed by Both PET and fMRI , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  M GöbelSilke,et al.  Response-Selection-Related Parietal Activation during Number Comparison , 2004 .

[17]  John C. Marshall,et al.  When Right Goes Left: An Investigation of Line Bisection in a Case of Visual Neglect , 1989, Cortex.

[18]  F. N. Dempster,et al.  Interference and inhibition in cognition: An historical perspective , 1995 .

[19]  Avishai Henik,et al.  A common representation for semantic and physical properties: a cognitive-anatomical approach. , 2006, Experimental psychology.

[20]  Gereon R. Fink,et al.  Deriving Numerosity and Shape from Identical Visual Displays , 2001, NeuroImage.

[21]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Cerebral Pathways for Calculation: Double Dissociation between Rote Verbal and Quantitative Knowledge of Arithmetic , 1997, Cortex.

[22]  Patrik Vuilleumier,et al.  The Number Space and Neglect , 2004, Cortex.

[23]  S. Dehaene,et al.  A Magnitude Code Common to Numerosities and Number Symbols in Human Intraparietal Cortex , 2007, Neuron.

[24]  Vincent Walsh A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[25]  Katsumi Aoki,et al.  Recent development of flow visualization , 2004, J. Vis..

[26]  C. Caltagirone,et al.  Perceiving numbers alters time perception , 2008, Neuroscience Letters.

[27]  Michael Andres,et al.  Number magnitude potentiates action judgements , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Mental rotation of objects versus hands: neural mechanisms revealed by positron emission tomography. , 1998, Psychophysiology.

[29]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[30]  M Corbetta,et al.  Frontoparietal cortical networks for directing attention and the eye to visual locations: identical, independent, or overlapping neural systems? , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[31]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Abstract representations of numbers in the animal and human brain , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.

[32]  ROBERT S. MOYER,et al.  Time required for Judgements of Numerical Inequality , 1967, Nature.

[33]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Discrete and analogue quantity processing in the parietal lobe: a functional MRI study. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[34]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Topographical Layout of Hand, Eye, Calculation, and Language-Related Areas in the Human Parietal Lobe , 2002, Neuron.

[35]  Sheng He,et al.  Larger stimuli are judged to last longer. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[36]  Olivier Houdé,et al.  Negative Priming in a Numerical Piaget-like Task as Evidenced by ERP , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[37]  Soon Chun Siong,et al.  Parametric effects of numerical distance on the intraparietal sulcus during passive viewing of rapid numerosity changes , 2006, Brain Research.

[38]  Nicolas Costes,et al.  Task-independent semantic activation for numbers and animals. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[39]  Michael W. L. Chee,et al.  Neural correlates of symbolic and non-symbolic arithmetic , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  E. Bisiach,et al.  Timing of right parietal and frontal cortex activity in visuo-spatial perception: a TMS study in normal individuals , 2001, Neuroreport.

[41]  D. Ansari Effects of development and enculturation on number representation in the brain , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[42]  P Dixon,et al.  Numerical comparison processes , 1978, Memory & cognition.

[43]  G A Orban,et al.  Orientation discrimination of objects and gratings compared: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[44]  D. LeBihan,et al.  Modulation of Parietal Activation by Semantic Distance in a Number Comparison Task , 2001, NeuroImage.

[45]  Timothy Edward John Behrens,et al.  Response-Selection-Related Parietal Activation during Number Comparison , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[46]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Neural Evidence Linking Visual Object Enumeration and Attention , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[47]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .

[48]  Michael Andres,et al.  Dissociation of numerosity and duration processing in the left intraparietal sulcus: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study , 2008, Cortex.

[49]  J. Piaget The Child's Conception of Number , 1953 .

[50]  Michael Andres,et al.  Number magnitude and grip aperture interaction , 2004, Neuroreport.

[51]  Valérie Dormal,et al.  Numerosity-length interference: a Stroop experiment. , 2007, Experimental psychology.

[52]  M. Sugishita,et al.  Isolated acalculia due to left parietal lesion. , 1994, Archives of neurology.

[53]  Michael S. Gazzaniga,et al.  Interview: Mark D'Esposito with Michael S. Gazzaniga , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[54]  Philippe Pinel,et al.  Tuning Curves for Approximate Numerosity in the Human Intraparietal Sulcus , 2004, Neuron.

[55]  A. Henik,et al.  Is three greater than five: The relation between physical and semantic size in comparison tasks , 1982, Memory & cognition.

[56]  M. Thioux,et al.  Neuroanatomical Substrates of Arabic Number Processing, Numerical Comparison, and Simple Addition: A PET Study , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[57]  Olivier Houdeé,et al.  Negative priming effect after inhibition of number/length interference in a Piaget-like task , 2001 .

[58]  Philippe Pinel,et al.  Distributed and Overlapping Cerebral Representations of Number, Size, and Luminance during Comparative Judgments , 2004, Neuron.

[59]  K. Zilles,et al.  Line bisection judgments implicate right parietal cortex and cerebellum as assessed by fMRI , 2000, Neurology.

[60]  B. Mazoyer,et al.  Neural Correlates of Simple and Complex Mental Calculation , 2001, NeuroImage.

[61]  W Fias,et al.  Irrelevant digits affect feature-based attention depending on the overlap of neural circuits. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[62]  K. Priftis,et al.  Brain damage: Neglect disrupts the mental number line , 2002, Nature.

[63]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Age-related Changes in the Activation of the Intraparietal Sulcus during Nonsymbolic Magnitude Processing: An Event-related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[64]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Are Subitizing and Counting Implemented as Separate or Functionally Overlapping Processes? , 2002, NeuroImage.

[65]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Event-related fMRI analysis of the cerebral circuit for number comparison. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[66]  Kyoung-Min Lee Cortical areas differentially involved in multiplication and subtraction: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study and correlation with a case of selective acalculia , 2000, Annals of neurology.

[67]  E. J. Carter,et al.  Functional Imaging of Numerical Processing in Adults and 4-y-Old Children , 2006, PLoS biology.

[68]  David J. Ostry,et al.  Time course of number magnitude interference during grasping , 2008, Cortex.

[69]  A. Cowey,et al.  A distortion of perceived space in patients withright-hemisphere lesions and visual hemineglect , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[70]  Andreas Nieder,et al.  Neuronal population coding of continuous and discrete quantity in the primate posterior parietal cortex , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.