Determinants of asynchronous processing in vision

When a stimulus oscillates in both colour and direction of motion, changes in colour must lag behind those in direction if they are to be seen as concurrent. It has been argued that this lag is the consequence of asynchronous visual processing, with colour being processed more rapidly than motion. This proposal is contentious: it has been criticized on the basis that the time‐course of cortical activity may not correlate directly with that of perceptual experience. Here, we demonstrate that the extent of the apparent asynchrony can vary according to the prevailing stimulus conditions. The apparent asynchrony is greatest if the stimulus is composed of opponent directions of motion and is reduced if the angular difference between the directions is reduced. This pattern of results suggests that asynchronous neural activity arises, in part, as a consequence of differential levels of inhibition within relatively independent cortical structures.

[1]  D. Hubel,et al.  Segregation of form, color, movement, and depth: anatomy, physiology, and perception. , 1988, Science.

[2]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  A minimum motion technique for judging equiluminance , 1983 .

[3]  N. Mai,et al.  Selective disturbance of movement vision after bilateral brain damage. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[4]  S. Nishida,et al.  Time perception: Brain time or event time? , 2001, Current Biology.

[5]  P Girard,et al.  Visual latencies in cytochrome oxidase bands of macaque area V2. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[6]  P. Schiller,et al.  Functional specificity of lateral geniculate nucleus laminae of the rhesus monkey. , 1978, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  R A Andersen,et al.  The response of area MT and V1 neurons to transparent motion , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[8]  P. Lennie Single Units and Visual Cortical Organization , 1998, Perception.

[9]  T J Sejnowski,et al.  Motion integration and postdiction in visual awareness. , 2000, Science.

[10]  S. Zeki,et al.  Toward a Theory of Visual Consciousness , 1999, Consciousness and Cognition.

[11]  H. Wässle,et al.  Response latency of brisk‐sustained (X) and brisk‐transient (Y) cells in the cat retina , 1982, The Journal of physiology.

[12]  D. Spalding The Principles of Psychology , 1873, Nature.

[13]  H. Barlow,et al.  The mechanism of directionally selective units in rabbit's retina. , 1965, The Journal of physiology.

[14]  C. N. Guy,et al.  The parallel visual motion inputs into areas V1 and V5 of human cerebral cortex. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[15]  D. J. Felleman,et al.  Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. , 1991, Cerebral cortex.

[16]  N. Daw Colour‐coded ganglion cells in the goldfish retina: extension of their receptive fields by means of new stimuli , 1968, The Journal of physiology.

[17]  Derek H. Arnold,et al.  Asynchronous processing in vision Color leads motion , 2001, Current Biology.

[18]  S. Zeki,et al.  The asynchrony of consciousness , 1998, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[19]  L. Maffei,et al.  Neural Correlate of Perceptual Adaptation to Gratings , 1973, Science.

[20]  I. Murakami,et al.  Latency difference, not spatial extrapolation , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  J. Movshon,et al.  Linearity and Normalization in Simple Cells of the Macaque Primary Visual Cortex , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  P. Schiller,et al.  Composition of geniculostriate input ot superior colliculus of the rhesus monkey. , 1979, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  S. Zeki,et al.  A direct demonstration of perceptual asynchrony in vision , 1997, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[24]  Semir Zeki,et al.  The theory of multistage integration in the visual brain , 1998, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[25]  S. Zeki Functional specialisation in the visual cortex of the rhesus monkey , 1978, Nature.

[26]  M C Corballis,et al.  Motion Perception: A Color-Contingent Aftereffect , 1972, Science.

[27]  Romi Nijhawan,et al.  Motion extrapolation in catching , 1994, Nature.

[28]  K. Moutoussis,et al.  Functional segregation and temporal hierarchy of the visual perceptive systems , 1997, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[29]  A. Cowey,et al.  Cerebral achromatopsia: colour blindness despite wavelength processing , 1997, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[30]  B. Richmond,et al.  Latency: another potential code for feature binding in striate cortex. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Visual response latencies of magnocellular and parvocellular LGN neurons in macaque monkeys , 1999, Visual Neuroscience.