What Is a Language? Who Is Bilingual? Perceptions Underlying Self-Assessment in Studies of Bilingualism

Research on the cognitive consequences of bilingualism typically proceeds by labeling participants as “monolingual” or “bilingual” and comparing performance on some measures across these groups. It is well-known that this approach has led to inconsistent results. However, the approach assumes that there are clear criteria to designate individuals as monolingual or bilingual, and more fundamentally, to determine whether a communication system counts as a unique language. Both of these assumptions may not be correct. The problem is particularly acute when participants are asked to classify themselves or simply report how many languages they speak. Participants' responses to these questions are shaped by their personal perceptions of the criteria for making these judgments. This study investigated the perceptions underlying judgments of bilingualism by asking 528 participants to judge the extent to which a description of a fictional linguistic system constitutes a unique language and the extent to which a description of a fictional individual's linguistic competence qualifies that person as bilingual. The results show a range of responses for both concepts, indicating substantial ambiguity for these terms. Moreover, participants were asked to self-classify as monolingual or bilingual, and these decisions were not related to more objective information regarding the degree of bilingual experience obtained from a detailed questionnaire. These results are consistent with the notion that bilingualism is not categorical and that specific language experiences are important in determining the criteria for being bilingual. The results impact interpretations of research investigating group differences on the cognitive effects of bilingualism.

[1]  V. Kempe,et al.  Language control in regional dialect speakers – monolingual by name, bilingual by nature? , 2021, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.

[2]  P. Kałamała,et al.  On the multidimensionality of bilingualism and the unique role of language use , 2021, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.

[3]  J. Annoni,et al.  Language distance modulates cognitive control in bilinguals , 2021, Scientific Reports.

[4]  John A. E. Anderson,et al.  What Have We Learned About Bilingualism? Regarding Nichols et al. (2020) , 2021 .

[5]  Cassandra J. Lowe,et al.  The Bilingual Advantage in Children’s Executive Functioning Is Not Related to Language Status: A Meta-Analytic Review , 2021, Psychological science.

[6]  Stéphanie Bellocchi,et al.  Is bilingualism associated with better working memory capacity? A meta-analysis , 2021, International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism.

[7]  E. Bialystok,et al.  Subtle Increments in Socioeconomic Status and Bilingualism Jointly Affect Children’s Verbal and Nonverbal Performance , 2021 .

[8]  Leslie R. Morris,et al.  Why Not Both? , 2020, Cosmological Conundrums.

[9]  Alissa Melinger Do elevators compete with lifts?: Selecting dialect alternatives , 2020, Cognition.

[10]  E. Bialystok Null results in bilingualism research , 2020 .

[11]  J. Grundy The effects of bilingualism on executive functions: an updated quantitative analysis , 2020, Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science.

[12]  Qiming Yuan,et al.  Language context tunes brain network for language control in bilingual language production , 2020, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  M. Kirkovski,et al.  Meta-Analysis Reveals a Bilingual Advantage That Is Dependent on Task and Age , 2020, Frontiers in Psychology.

[14]  R. Mishra,et al.  High proficient bilinguals bring in higher executive control when encountering diverse interlocutors , 2020 .

[15]  J. Rothman,et al.  On the phantom-like appearance of bilingualism effects on neurocognition: (How) should we proceed? , 2020, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.

[16]  A. Owen,et al.  Bilingualism Affords No General Cognitive Advantages: A Population Study of Executive Function in 11,000 People , 2020, Psychological science.

[17]  K. Byers‐Heinlein,et al.  Why not both? Rethinking categorical and continuous approaches to bilingualism , 2020, The international journal of bilingualism : cross-disciplinary, cross-linguistic studies of language behavior.

[18]  Kenneth Hyltenstam,et al.  Age of acquisition – not bilingualism – is the primary determinant of less than nativelike L2 ultimate attainment , 2020, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.

[19]  Johan Gross,et al.  Self-assessment and standard language ideologies: bilingual adolescents in Sweden reflect on their language proficiencies , 2020 .

[20]  Jason Rothman,et al.  Duration and extent of bilingual experience modulate neurocognitive outcomes , 2020, NeuroImage.

[21]  C. Vorwerg,et al.  Language control and lexical access in diglossic speech production: Evidence from variety switching in speakers of Swiss German , 2019, Journal of Memory and Language.

[22]  T. Oh,et al.  Age and Proficiency in the Bilingual Brain Revisited: Activation Patterns Across Different L2-Learner Types , 2019, Front. Commun..

[23]  J. Kroll,et al.  English only? Monolinguals in linguistically diverse contexts have an edge in language learning , 2019, Brain and Language.

[24]  Raphael Berthele,et al.  The effect of bidialectalism on executive function , 2019 .

[25]  Jason Rothman,et al.  Redefining bilingualism as a spectrum of experiences that differentially affects brain structure and function , 2019, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[26]  P. J. Brooks,et al.  Is there a bilingual advantage on interference-control tasks? A multiverse meta-analysis of global reaction time and interference cost , 2019, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[27]  D. Titone,et al.  Characterizing the social diversity of bilingualism using language entropy , 2019, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.

[28]  K. Vermeire,et al.  Does the Bilingual Advantage in Cognitive Control Exist and If So, What Are Its Modulating Factors? A Systematic Review , 2019, Behavioral sciences.

[29]  Andree Hartanto,et al.  Does Early Active Bilingualism Enhance Inhibitory Control and Monitoring? A Propensity-Matching Analysis , 2019, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[30]  M. Antoniou The Advantages of Bilingualism Debate , 2019, Annual Review of Linguistics.

[31]  Kelly A. Vaughn,et al.  Symbiosis, Parasitism and Bilingual Cognitive Control: A Neuroemergentist Perspective , 2018, Front. Psychol..

[32]  W. Sandler The Body as Evidence for the Nature of Language , 2018, Front. Psychol..

[33]  Susanne Carroll,et al.  Language and Situation , 2018 .

[34]  Paola E. Dussias,et al.  Language Use Across International Contexts: Shaping the Minds of L2 Speakers , 2018, Annual Review of Applied Linguistics.

[35]  Ø. Vangsnes,et al.  Language Separation in Bidialectal Speakers: Evidence From Eye Tracking , 2018, Front. Psychol..

[36]  V. Ferreira,et al.  When a seven is not a seven: Self-ratings of bilingual language proficiency differ between and within language populations , 2018, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.

[37]  Soudabeh Nour,et al.  A domain-general monitoring account of language switching in recognition tasks: Evidence for adaptive control , 2018, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.

[38]  J. Kroll,et al.  Bilingualism reveals fundamental variation in language processing , 2018, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.

[39]  Debra Titone,et al.  Bilingual experience and resting-state brain connectivity: Impacts of L2 age of acquisition and social diversity of language use on control networks , 2018, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  K. de Bot,et al.  Intensity of Multilingual Language Use Predicts Cognitive Performance in Some Multilingual Older Adults , 2018, Brain sciences.

[41]  Ruiming Wang,et al.  Bilingual Contexts Modulate the Inhibitory Control Network , 2018, Front. Psychol..

[42]  David Birdsong Plasticity, Variability and Age in Second Language Acquisition and Bilingualism , 2018, Front. Psychol..

[43]  Minna Lehtonen,et al.  Is Bilingualism Associated With Enhanced Executive Functioning in Adults? A Meta-Analytic Review , 2018, Psychological bulletin.

[44]  Alissa Melinger Distinguishing languages from dialects: A litmus test using the picture-word interference task , 2018, Cognition.

[45]  Ellen Bialystok,et al.  The language and social background questionnaire: Assessing degree of bilingualism in a diverse population , 2018, Behavior research methods.

[46]  G. Luk,et al.  Describing bilinguals: A systematic review of labels and descriptions used in the literature between 2005–2015 , 2017, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.

[47]  F. Genesee,et al.  The effects of age of acquisition on verbal memory in bilinguals , 2017 .

[48]  X. Chai,et al.  Simultaneous learning of two languages from birth positively impacts intrinsic functional connectivity and cognitive control , 2017, Brain and Cognition.

[49]  Zhang Jijia,et al.  Effects of “each speaks their own dialect” phenomenon on the executive function of Jingpo students , 2017 .

[50]  J. Grundy,et al.  Bilingualism and working memory capacity: A comprehensive meta-analysis , 2017 .

[51]  Theodoros Marinis,et al.  Effects of dense code-switching on executive control , 2016 .

[52]  C. Vaidya,et al.  Do Bilingual advantages in attentional control influence memory encoding during a divided attention task? , 2016, Bilingualism.

[53]  A. Ardila,et al.  The effect of language proficiency on executive functions in balanced and unbalanced Spanish–English bilinguals * , 2016 .

[54]  T. Bak The impact of bilinguism on cognitive aging and dementia , 2015 .

[55]  S. Della Sala,et al.  Cognitive Advantage in Bilingualism , 2015, Psychological science.

[56]  K. Paap,et al.  Bilingual advantages in executive functioning: problems in convergent validity, discriminant validity, and the identification of the theoretical constructs , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[57]  D. Titone,et al.  Moving toward a neuroplasticity view of bilingualism, executive control, and aging , 2014, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[58]  Clara D. Martin,et al.  Where is the bilingual advantage in task-switching? , 2013 .

[59]  E. Bialystok,et al.  Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage , 2013, Journal of cognitive psychology.

[60]  D. Green,et al.  Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis , 2013, Journal of cognitive psychology.

[61]  K. Paap,et al.  There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing , 2013, Cognitive Psychology.

[62]  E. Bialystok,et al.  Is there a relation between onset age of bilingualism and enhancement of cognitive control?* , 2011, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.

[63]  Tracy A. Lavin,et al.  A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of the Cognitive Correlates of Bilingualism , 2010 .

[64]  B. MacWhinney,et al.  A Bilingual Advantage in Task Switching * Anat Prior , 2010 .

[65]  Margarita Kaushanskaya,et al.  The bilingual advantage in novel word learning , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[66]  E. Bialystok,et al.  Bilingualism across the lifespan: The rise and fall of inhibitory control , 2005 .

[67]  Kyriakos Antoniou,et al.  An Investigation of the Multilingual and Bi-dialectal Advantage in Executive Control , 2020, CogSci.

[68]  N. Katsos,et al.  How does childhood bilingualism and bi-dialectalism affect the interpretation and processing of pragmatic meanings? , 2019, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.

[69]  E. Bialystok The signal and the noise: Finding the pattern in human behavior , 2016 .

[70]  S. Sala,et al.  Cognitive advantage in bilingualism: An example of publication bias? , 2015 .

[71]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[72]  Sandra Merz Bott Speech Intelligibility and Bilingualism: The Effects of Age of Acquisition , 1993 .

[73]  刘润清 Language and Situation:Language varieties and their social contexts , 1984 .