Using 1/f noise to examine planning and control in a discrete aiming task

The present study used 1/f noise to examine how spatial, physical, and timing constraints affect planning and control processes in aiming. Participants moved objects of different masses to different distances at preferred speed (Experiment 1) and as quickly as possible (Experiment 2). Power spectral density, standardized dispersion, rescaled range, and an autoregressive fractionally integrated moving average (ARFIMA) model selection procedure were used to quantify 1/f noise. Measures from all four analyses were in reasonable agreement, with more ARFIMA (long-range) models selected at peak velocity in Experiment 1 and fewer selected at peak velocity in Experiment 2. There also was a nonsignificant trend where, at preferred speed, of those participants who showed 1/f noise, more tended to show 1/f noise at peak velocity, when planning and control would overlap most. This trend disappeared for fast movements, where planning and control would have less time to overlap. Summing short-range processes at different timescales can produce 1/f-like noise. As planning is a slower-moving process and control faster, present results suggest that, with enough time for both planning and control, 1/f noise in aiming may arise from a similar summation of processes. Potential limitations of time series length in the present task are discussed.

[1]  Jeffrey M. Hausdorff,et al.  Fractal dynamics of human gait: stability of long-range correlations in stride interval fluctuations. , 1996, Journal of applied physiology.

[2]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Estimation and interpretation of 1/fα noise in human cognition , 2004 .

[3]  K. Newell,et al.  Movement time and velocity as determinants of movement timing accuracy. , 1979, Journal of motor behavior.

[4]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Human cognition and a pile of sand: a discussion on serial correlations and self-organized criticality. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[5]  J. Bassingthwaighte,et al.  Evaluation of the dispersional analysis method for fractal time series , 1995, Annals of Biomedical Engineering.

[6]  Marius Ooms,et al.  A Package for Estimating, Forecasting and Simulating Arfima Models: Arfima package 1.0 for Ox , 1999 .

[7]  G. V. van Orden,et al.  Self-organization of cognitive performance. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[8]  Yanqing Chen,et al.  Long Memory Processes ( 1 / f α Type) in Human Coordination , 1997 .

[9]  Masashi Yamada,et al.  Temporal control mechanism of repetitive tapping with simple rhythmic patterns , 2001 .

[10]  Mingzhou Ding,et al.  Statistical Analysis of Timing Errors , 2002, Brain and Cognition.

[11]  A. Eke,et al.  Fractal characterization of complexity in temporal physiological signals , 2002, Physiological measurement.

[12]  C. Ghez,et al.  Trajectory control in targeted force impulses , 1987, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  A. Craig Spinocervical tract cells in cat and dog, labeled by the retrograde transport of horseradish peroxidase , 1976, Neuroscience Letters.

[14]  D. Percival,et al.  Physiological time series , 2000 .

[15]  K. Torre,et al.  Detection of long-range dependence and estimation of fractal exponents through ARFIMA modelling. , 2007, The British journal of mathematical and statistical psychology.

[16]  Steven W. Keele,et al.  Movement control in skilled motor performance. , 1968 .

[17]  Matthew Heath,et al.  The control of goal-directed limb movements: Correcting errors in the trajectory , 1999 .

[18]  S. Glover,et al.  Separate visual representations in the planning and control of action , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[19]  D. Elliott,et al.  The influence of advance information about target location and visual feedback on movement planning and execution. , 2006, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[20]  D. Percival,et al.  Physiological time series: distinguishing fractal noises from motions , 2000, Pflügers Archiv.

[21]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  An internal model for sensorimotor integration. , 1995, Science.

[22]  W. Helsen,et al.  A century later: Woodworth's (1899) two-component model of goal-directed aiming. , 2001, Psychological bulletin.

[23]  G. Fullerton Psychology and physiology. , 1896 .

[24]  E. Wagenmakers,et al.  AIC model selection using Akaike weights , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[25]  B. Weiss,et al.  A stochastic medelfor time-ordered dependencies in continous scale repetitive judgments. , 1955, Journal of experimental psychology.

[26]  G. Madison,et al.  Variability in isochronous tapping: higher order dependencies as a function of intertap interval. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  P. Killeen Complex dynamic processes in sign tracking with an omission contingency (negative automaintenance). , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[28]  R. Adler,et al.  A practical guide to heavy tails: statistical techniques and applications , 1998 .

[29]  H. Akaike,et al.  Information Theory and an Extension of the Maximum Likelihood Principle , 1973 .

[30]  Robert Sessions Woodworth,et al.  THE ACCURACY OF VOLUNTARY MOVEMENT , 1899 .

[31]  Rangarajan,et al.  Integrated approach to the assessment of long range correlation in time series data , 2000, Physical review. E, Statistical physics, plasmas, fluids, and related interdisciplinary topics.

[32]  Thomas L. Thornton,et al.  Provenance of correlations in psychological data , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[33]  W. J. Merrill,et al.  The application of temporal correlation techniques in psychology. , 1956 .

[34]  Tatsuyuki Ohtsuki,et al.  Fractal correlation of initial trajectory dynamics vanishes at the movement end point in human rapid goal-directed movements , 2001, Neuroscience Letters.

[35]  K. Torre,et al.  Fractal analyses for 'short' time series: A re-assessment of classical methods , 2006 .

[36]  Turalay Kenc,et al.  Ox: An Object-Oriented Matrix Language , 1997 .

[37]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[38]  Bruce J. West,et al.  Hölder exponent spectra for human gait , 2002, cond-mat/0208028.

[39]  T. Musha,et al.  Fluctuations of Human Tapping Intervals , 1985, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[40]  Zoubin Ghahramani,et al.  Computational principles of movement neuroscience , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[41]  Luc Tremblay,et al.  Online Control of Discrete Action following Visual Perturbation , 2007, Perception.

[42]  Physiological Heterogeneity: Fractals Link Determinism and Randomness in Structures and Functions , 1988 .

[43]  Murad S. Taqqu,et al.  On estimating the intensity of long-range dependence in finite and infinite variance time series , 1998 .

[44]  James L. Lyons,et al.  Optimal Control Strategies Under Different Feedback Schedules: Kinematic Evidence , 2002, Journal of motor behavior.

[45]  M Yamada,et al.  Temporal control mechanism in equaled interval tapping. , 1996, Applied human science : journal of physiological anthropology.

[46]  W. S. Verplanck,et al.  Nonindependence of successive responses in measurements of the visual threshold. , 1952, Journal of experimental psychology.

[47]  D. Percival,et al.  Analyzing exact fractal time series: evaluating dispersional analysis and rescaled range methods. , 1997, Physica A.

[48]  Luc Tremblay,et al.  Learning to Optimize Speed, Accuracy, and Energy Expenditure: A Framework for Understanding Speed-Accuracy Relations in Goal-Directed Aiming , 2004, Journal of motor behavior.

[49]  D Laming,et al.  Autocorrelation of choice-reaction times. , 1979, Acta psychologica.

[50]  L. Liebovitch,et al.  "Fractal dynamics of human gait: stability of long-range correlations in stride interval fluctuations". , 1996, Journal of applied physiology.

[51]  J. B. J. Smeets,et al.  Adjustments of fast goal-directed movements in response to an unexpected inertial load , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[52]  Donald Laming,et al.  Information theory of choice-reaction times , 1968 .

[53]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Comparison of variability of initial kinematics and endpoints of reaching movements , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[54]  J. Holden CHAPTER 6 Gauging the Fractal Dimension of Response Times from Cognitive Tasks , 2004 .

[55]  R Plamondon,et al.  Speed/accuracy trade-offs in target-directed movements , 1997, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[56]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  1/f noise in human cognition: Is it ubiquitous, and what does it mean? , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[57]  Timothy D. Lee,et al.  Motor Control and Learning: A Behavioral Emphasis , 1982 .