Altered Emotional Interference Processing in Affective and Cognitive-Control Brain Circuitry in Major Depression

BACKGROUND Major depression is characterized by a negativity bias: an enhanced responsiveness to, and memory for, affectively negative stimuli. However, it is not yet clear whether this bias represents 1) impaired top-down cognitive control over affective responses, potentially linked to deficits in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex function; or 2) enhanced bottom-up responses to affectively laden stimuli that dysregulate cognitive control mechanisms, potentially linked to deficits in amygdala and anterior cingulate function. METHODS We used an attentional interference task using emotional distracters to test for top-down versus bottom-up dysfunction in the interaction of cognitive-control circuitry and emotion-processing circuitry. A total of 27 patients with major depression and 24 control participants was tested. Event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging was carried out as participants directly attended to, or attempted to ignore, fear-related stimuli. RESULTS Compared with control subjects, patients with depression showed an enhanced amygdala response to unattended fear-related stimuli (relative to unattended neutral). By contrast, control participants showed increased activity in right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Brodmann areas 46/9) when ignoring fear stimuli (relative to neutral), which the patients with depression did not show. In addition, the depressed participants failed to show evidence of error-related cognitive adjustments (increased activity in bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal cortex on posterror trials), but the control group did show them. CONCLUSIONS These results suggest multiple sources of dysregulation in emotional and cognitive control circuitry in depression, implicating both top-down and bottom-up dysfunction.

[1]  W. Drevets Functional anatomical abnormalities in limbic and prefrontal cortical structures in major depression. , 2000, Progress in brain research.

[2]  Kevin N. Ochsner,et al.  For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion , 2004, NeuroImage.

[3]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A neuromodulatory role for the human amygdala in processing emotional facial expressions. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[4]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The neural basis of error detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. , 2004, Psychological review.

[5]  J. Duncan,et al.  Prefrontal cortical function and anxiety: controlling attention to threat-related stimuli , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[6]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[7]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Separating Processes within a Trial in Event-Related Functional MRI II. Analysis , 2001, NeuroImage.

[8]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  Learned Predictions of Error Likelihood in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2005, Science.

[9]  A. Anderson,et al.  Neural Correlates of the Automatic Processing of Threat Facial Signals , 2022 .

[10]  Matthew Flatt,et al.  PsyScope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers , 1993 .

[11]  Erich O. Richter,et al.  Human Anterior Cingulate Cortex Neurons Encode Cognitive and Emotional Demands , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  D M Wegner,et al.  Depression and mental control: the resurgence of unwanted negative thoughts. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[13]  Yul-Wan Sung,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging , 2004, Scholarpedia.

[14]  Eliot Hazeltine,et al.  Dissociable Contributions of Prefrontal and Parietal Cortices to Response Selection , 2002, NeuroImage.

[15]  T. Braver,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of frequency, inhibition and errors. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[16]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. , 1998, Science.

[17]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Movement‐Related effects in fMRI time‐series , 1996, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[18]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Neural processing of emotional faces requires attention , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[19]  E. Donchin,et al.  Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[20]  R. Davidson,et al.  Depression: perspectives from affective neuroscience. , 2002, Annual review of psychology.

[21]  Mike Oaksford,et al.  Cognitive science, anxiety, and depression: from experiments to connectionism , 1992 .

[22]  K. Phan,et al.  Neural substrates for voluntary suppression of negative affect: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study , 2005, Biological Psychiatry.

[23]  Jeremy R. Reynolds,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Transient and Sustained Cognitive Control during Task Switching , 2003, Neuron.

[24]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.

[25]  John Duncan,et al.  State Anxiety Modulation of the Amygdala Response to Unattended Threat-Related Stimuli , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  P. Rabbitt Errors and error correction in choice-response tasks. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[27]  A. Lozano,et al.  Deep Brain Stimulation for Treatment-Resistant Depression , 2005, Neuron.

[28]  M. Mintun,et al.  Increased amygdala response to masked emotional faces in depressed subjects resolves with antidepressant treatment: an fMRI study , 2001, Biological Psychiatry.

[29]  D. Heeger,et al.  Linear Systems Analysis of Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging in Human V1 , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  I. Miller,et al.  Characteristics of depressed patients with elevated levels of dysfunctional cognitions , 1988, Cognitive Therapy and Research.

[31]  K. Mogg,et al.  Attentional bias in anxiety and depression: the role of awareness. , 1995, The British journal of clinical psychology.

[32]  M. Thase,et al.  Can’t shake that feeling: event-related fMRI assessment of sustained amygdala activity in response to emotional information in depressed individuals , 2002, Biological Psychiatry.

[33]  Marcus E. Raichle,et al.  Suppression of Regional Cerebral Blood during Emotional versus Higher Cognitive Implications for Interactions between Emotion and Cognition , 1998 .

[34]  M. Raichle,et al.  A functional anatomical study of unipolar depression , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[35]  M. Hamilton A RATING SCALE FOR DEPRESSION , 1960, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[36]  J. Gross,et al.  The cognitive control of emotion , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[37]  W. Drevets Neuroimaging and neuropathological studies of depression: implications for the cognitive-emotional features of mood disorders , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[38]  A. Damasio,et al.  Deciding Advantageously Before Knowing the Advantageous Strategy , 1997, Science.

[39]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Improved Assessment of Significant Activation in Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI): Use of a Cluster‐Size Threshold , 1995, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[40]  Jack L. Lancaster,et al.  A modality‐independent approach to spatial normalization of tomographic images of the human brain , 1995 .

[41]  M. Thase,et al.  Increased Amygdala and Decreased Dorsolateral Prefrontal BOLD Responses in Unipolar Depression: Related and Independent Features , 2007, Biological Psychiatry.

[42]  John Suckling,et al.  Attenuation of the neural response to sad faces in major depression by antidepressant treatment: a prospective, event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. , 2004, Archives of general psychiatry.

[43]  S. Carmichael,et al.  Networks related to the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex; a substrate for emotional behavior? , 1996, Progress in brain research.

[44]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[45]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Cytoarchitectonic definition of prefrontal areas in the normal human cortex: II. Variability in locations of areas 9 and 46 and relationship to the Talairach Coordinate System. , 1995, Cerebral cortex.

[46]  J. Price,et al.  The organization of networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys and humans. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[47]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Resting anterior cingulate activity and abnormal responses to errors in subjects with elevated depressive symptoms: A 128‐channel EEG study , 2006, Human brain mapping.

[48]  R. Dolan,et al.  Effects of Attention and Emotion on Face Processing in the Human Brain An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2001, Neuron.

[49]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[50]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Brain Systems Mediating Cognitive Interference by Emotional Distraction , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  L. Parsons,et al.  Reciprocal limbic-cortical function and negative mood: converging PET findings in depression and normal sadness. , 1999, The American journal of psychiatry.

[52]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Separating Processes within a Trial in Event-Related Functional MRI I. The Method , 2001, NeuroImage.