The P3b and P600(s): Positive contributions to language comprehension.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] M. Fabiani. It was the best of times, it was the worst of times: a psychophysiologist's view of cognitive aging. , 2012, Psychophysiology.
[2] M. Fabiani,et al. Individual differences in P3 scalp distribution in older adults, and their relationship to frontal lobe function. , 1998, Psychophysiology.
[3] Douglas Saddy,et al. Distinct Neurophysiological Patterns Reflecting Aspects of Syntactic Complexity and Syntactic Repair , 2002, Journal of psycholinguistic research.
[4] S. Kelly,et al. The classic P300 encodes a build‐to‐threshold decision variable , 2015, The European journal of neuroscience.
[5] G. Mulder,et al. When syntax meets semantics. , 1997, Psychophysiology.
[6] L. Osterhout,et al. Event-Related Brain Potentials Elicited by Failure to Agree , 1995 .
[7] N. Squires,et al. Two varieties of long-latency positive waves evoked by unpredictable auditory stimuli in man. , 1975, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[8] Hans-Jochen Heinze,et al. Brain potentials and syntactic violations revisited: no evidence for specificity of the syntactic positive shift , 1998, Neuropsychologia.
[9] E Donchin,et al. Averaged evoked potentials and intramodality selective attention. , 1967, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[10] V S Johnston,et al. The relationship between menstrual phase and the P3 component of ERPs. , 1991, Psychophysiology.
[11] C. Phillips,et al. No semantic illusions in the “Semantic P600” phenomenon: ERP evidence from Mandarin Chinese , 2013, Brain Research.
[12] Kara D. Federmeier,et al. It’s All in the Family , 2015, Psychological science.
[13] D. Tanner,et al. Individual Differences in the Real-Time Neural Dynamics of Language Comprehension , 2018 .
[14] M. Fabiani,et al. Changes in brain activity patterns in aging: the novelty oddball. , 1995, Psychophysiology.
[15] Matthew W. Crocker,et al. A Neurocomputational Model of the N400 and the P600 in Language Processing , 2016, Cognitive science.
[16] D Friedman,et al. An overview of age-related changes in the scalp distribution of P3b. , 1997, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[17] J. Polich,et al. P300 and the menstrual cycle. , 1988, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[18] S. Hillyard,et al. Long-latency evoked potentials to irrelevant, deviant stimuli. , 1976, Behavioral biology.
[19] E. Donchin. Presidential address, 1980. Surprise!...Surprise? , 1981, Psychophysiology.
[20] A. Friederici,et al. Differential task effects on semantic and syntactic processes as revealed by ERPs. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[21] Kara D. Federmeier,et al. Right words and left words: electrophysiological evidence for hemispheric differences in meaning processing. , 1999, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[22] M. Kutas,et al. Event-related brain potentials to grammatical errors and semantic anomalies , 1983, Memory & cognition.
[23] M. Kutas,et al. Parafoveal N400 effect during sentence reading , 2010, Neuroscience Letters.
[24] Matthias Schlesewsky,et al. An alternative perspective on “semantic P600” effects in language comprehension , 2008, Brain Research Reviews.
[25] Peter Ford Dominey,et al. Human brain potentials reveal similar processing of non-linguistic abstract structure and linguistic syntactic structure , 2002, Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology.
[26] C. Richard Clark,et al. Enlarged frontal P300 to stimulus change in panic disorder , 1996, Biological Psychiatry.
[27] Janet G. van Hell,et al. Dissociating retrieval interference and reanalysis in the P600 during sentence comprehension. , 2017, Psychophysiology.
[28] R. Seidl,et al. A taurine and caffeine-containing drink stimulates cognitive performance and well-being , 2000, Amino Acids.
[29] Colin M. Brown,et al. The syntactic positive shift (sps) as an erp measure of syntactic processing , 1993 .
[30] Vicka R. Corey,et al. On the Language Specificity of the Brain Response to Syntactic Anomalies: Is the Syntactic Positive Shift a Member of the P300 Family? , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[31] E. Donchin,et al. On quantifying surprise: the variation of event-related potentials with subjective probability. , 1977, Psychophysiology.
[32] Kara D. Federmeier,et al. Language of the Aging Brain: Event-Related Potential Studies of Comprehension in Older Adults , 2010, Lang. Linguistics Compass.
[33] Maryellen C. MacDonald,et al. The lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution , 1994 .
[34] ROBERT M. CHAPMAN,et al. Evoked Responses to Numerical and Non-Numerical Visual Stimuli while Problem Solving , 1964, Nature.
[35] Phillip J. Holcomb,et al. Two Neurocognitive Mechanisms of Semantic Integration during the Comprehension of Visual Real-world Events , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[36] J. Polich,et al. Meta-analysis of P300 and schizophrenia: patients, paradigms, and practical implications. , 2003, Psychophysiology.
[37] P. Holcomb,et al. Event-related brain potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly , 1992 .
[38] Hartmut Fitz,et al. Getting real about Semantic Illusions: Rethinking the functional role of the P600 in language comprehension , 2012, Brain Research.
[39] A. Friederici,et al. Electrophysiological Evidence for Two Steps in Syntactic Analysis: Early Automatic and Late Controlled Processes , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[40] Kara D. Federmeier,et al. Revisiting the incremental effects of context on word processing: Evidence from single-word event-related brain potentials. , 2015, Psychophysiology.
[41] E. Donchin,et al. P300 and recall in an incidental memory paradigm. , 1986, Psychophysiology.
[42] Joachim Röschke,et al. A Confirmatory Study on the Mechanisms Behind Reduced P300 Waves in Depression , 2003, Neuropsychopharmacology.
[43] E. Vogel,et al. Word meanings can be accessed but not reported during the attentional blink , 1996, Nature.
[44] H. Kolk,et al. An ERP study of P600 effects elicited by semantic anomalies. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[45] Marta Kutas,et al. Getting a cue before getting a clue: Event-related potentials to inference in visual narrative comprehension , 2015, Neuropsychologia.
[46] D. Caplan,et al. Electrophysiological distinctions in processing conceptual relationships within simple sentences. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[47] Kara D. Federmeier,et al. Finding the right word: Hemispheric asymmetries in the use of sentence context information , 2007, Neuropsychologia.
[48] Marta Kutas,et al. Syntactic processing with aging: an event-related potential study. , 2004, Psychophysiology.
[49] Kara D. Federmeier,et al. Age-related shifts in hemispheric dominance for syntactic processing. , 2017, Psychophysiology.
[50] M. Schlesewsky,et al. Parafoveal versus foveal N400s dissociate spreading activation from contextual fit , 2009, Neuroreport.
[51] Noam Chomsky,et al. The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? , 2002 .
[52] M. Kutas,et al. Interactions between sentence context and word frequencyinevent-related brainpotentials , 1990, Memory & cognition.
[53] T. Sejnowski,et al. Functionally Independent Components of the Late Positive Event-Related Potential during Visual Spatial Attention , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[54] Morten H. Christiansen,et al. Similar neural correlates for language and sequential learning: Evidence from event-related brain potentials , 2012, Language and cognitive processes.
[55] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Decision making, the P3, and the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.
[56] Kara D. Federmeier,et al. Thirty years and counting: finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). , 2011, Annual review of psychology.
[57] Kara D. Federmeier,et al. Out of the corner of my eye: Foveal semantic load modulates parafoveal processing in reading. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[58] E Donchin,et al. A metric for thought: a comparison of P300 latency and reaction time. , 1981, Science.
[59] Neil Cohn,et al. Electrophysiological Correlates of Complement Coercion , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[60] Kara D. Federmeier,et al. A Rose by Any Other Name: Long-Term Memory Structure and Sentence Processing , 1999 .
[61] A. Friederici,et al. Why the P600 is not just a P300: the role of the basal ganglia , 2003, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[62] Kim Ainsworth-Darnell,et al. Dissociating Brain Responses to Syntactic and Semantic Anomalies: Evidence from Event-Related Potentials , 1998 .
[63] Neil Cohn,et al. The grammar of visual narrative: Neural evidence for constituent structure in sequential image comprehension , 2014, Neuropsychologia.
[64] M. Schlesewsky,et al. The P600-as-P3 hypothesis revisited: Single-trial analyses reveal that the late EEG positivity following linguistically deviant material is reaction time aligned , 2014, Brain and Language.
[65] A. Kok. On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. , 2001, Psychophysiology.
[66] G. McCarthy,et al. Augmenting mental chronometry: the P300 as a measure of stimulus evaluation time. , 1977, Science.
[67] M. Kutas,et al. Expect the Unexpected: Event-related Brain Response to Morphosyntactic Violations , 1998 .
[68] Phillip J. Holcomb,et al. Neural correlates of processing syntactic, semantic, and thematic relationships in sentences , 2006 .
[69] S A Hillyard,et al. P3 waves to the discrimination of targets in homogeneous and heterogeneous stimulus sequences. , 1977, Psychophysiology.
[70] M. Hansenne,et al. Le potentiel évoqué cognitif P300 (I) : aspects théorique et psychobiologique , 2000, Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology.
[71] J. Wolfe,et al. Differential Electrophysiological Signatures of Semantic and Syntactic Scene Processing , 2013, Psychological science.
[72] R. Verleger. Event-related potentials and cognition: A critique of the context updating hypothesis and an alternative interpretation of P3 , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[73] J. Polich. Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[74] A. Friederici,et al. Temporal structure of syntactic parsing: early and late event-related brain potential effects. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[75] E. John,et al. Evoked-Potential Correlates of Stimulus Uncertainty , 1965, Science.
[76] Gina R. Kuperberg,et al. Neural mechanisms of language comprehension: Challenges to syntax , 2007, Brain Research.
[77] C. Phillips,et al. Examining the evidence for an independent semantic analyzer: An ERP study in Spanish , 2012, Brain and Language.
[78] Colin M. Brown,et al. ERP effects of listening to speech compared to reading: the P600/SPS to syntactic violations in spoken sentences and rapid serial visual presentation , 2000, Neuropsychologia.
[79] N. Squires,et al. The effect of stimulus sequence on the waveform of the cortical event-related potential. , 1976, Science.
[80] L. Osterhout,et al. Brain potentials elicited by prose-embedded linguistic anomalies , 2002, Memory & cognition.
[81] M. Garrett,et al. Syntactically Based Sentence Processing Classes: Evidence from Event-Related Brain Potentials , 1991, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[82] M. Kutas,et al. Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. , 1980, Science.
[83] Dorothee J. Chwilla,et al. When Heuristics Clash with Parsing Routines: ERP Evidence for Conflict Monitoring in Sentence Perception , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[84] R. Knight,et al. Neural origins of the P300. , 2000, Critical reviews in neurobiology.
[85] Monica Fabiani,et al. Putting things into perspective: individual differences in working-memory span and the integration of information. , 2005, Experimental psychology.
[86] D. Tanner,et al. ERPs reveal individual differences in morphosyntactic processing , 2014, Neuropsychologia.
[87] Peter Hagoort,et al. Real-time semantic compensation in patients with agrammatic comprehension: Electrophysiological evidence for multiple-route plasticity , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[88] J. Polich. Neuropsychology of P300 , 2011 .
[89] H. Kolk,et al. Structure and limited capacity in verbal working memory: A study with event-related potentials , 2003, Brain and Language.
[90] C. Van Petten,et al. Prediction during language comprehension: benefits, costs, and ERP components. , 2012, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[91] E. Gibson,et al. The P600 as an index of syntactic integration difficulty , 2000 .
[92] L. Osterhout,et al. The independence of combinatory semantic processing: Evidence from event-related potentials , 2005 .
[93] Peter Hagoort,et al. About Edible Restaurants: Conflicts between Syntax and Semantics as Revealed by ERPs , 2010, Front. Psychology.
[94] K. Rayner,et al. Making and correcting errors during sentence comprehension: Eye movements in the analysis of structurally ambiguous sentences , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.
[95] Aniruddh D. Patel,et al. Processing Syntactic Relations in Language and Music: An Event-Related Potential Study , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[96] K. Morita,et al. Effects of caffeine on event‐related potentials: Comparison of oddball with single‐tone paradigms , 1996, Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences.