On the relation between object shape and grasping kinematics.

Despite the many studies on the visual control of grasping, little is known about how and when small variations in shape affect grasping kinematics. In the present study we asked subjects to grasp elliptical cylinders that were placed 30 and 60 cm in front of them. The cylinders' aspect ratio was varied systematically between 0.4 and 1.6, and their orientation was varied in steps of 30 degrees. Subjects picked up all noncircular cylinders with a hand orientation that approximately coincided with one of the principal axes. The probability of selecting a given principal axis was the highest when its orientation was equal to the preferred orientation for picking up a circular cylinder at the same location. The maximum grip aperture was scaled to the length of the selected principal axis, but the maximum grip aperture was also larger when the length of the axis orthogonal to the grip axis was longer than that of the grip axis. The correlation between the grip aperture--or the hand orientation--at a given instant, and its final value, increased monotonically with the traversed distance. The final hand orientation could already be inferred from its value after 30% of the movement distance with a reliability that explains 50% of the variance. For the final grip aperture, this was only so after 80% of the movement distance. The results indicate that the perceived shape of the cylinder is used for selecting appropriate grasping locations before or early in the movement and that the grip aperture and orientation are gradually attuned to these locations during the movement.

[1]  E. Johnston Systematic distortions of shape from stereopsis , 1991, Vision Research.

[2]  P. Mamassian Prehension of objects oriented in three-dimensional space , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  D. Pélisson,et al.  From Eye to Hand: Planning Goal-directed Movements , 1998, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[4]  Rachid Deriche,et al.  Regularization, Scale-Space, and Edge Detection Filters , 1996, ECCV.

[5]  Peter Dixon,et al.  Motor adaptation to an optical illusion , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[6]  J. Koenderink The structure of images , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[7]  A. Roby-Brami,et al.  Coupling between reaching movement direction and hand orientation for grasping , 2002, Brain Research.

[8]  Raymond H. Cuijpers,et al.  Illusions in action: consequences of inconsistent processing of spatial attributes , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Selective perturbation of visual input during prehension movements , 1991, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  J Richardson,et al.  Spatial patterns in the control of human arm movement. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  M. Jeannerod Intersegmental coordination during reaching at natural visual objects , 1981 .

[12]  Alan Wing,et al.  Coordination of hand aperture with the spatial path of hand transport , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  Eli Brenner,et al.  PII: S0042-6989(98)00162-X , 1998 .

[14]  J T Todd,et al.  Distortions of Three-Dimensional Space in the Perceptual Analysis of Motion and Stereo , 1995, Perception.

[15]  Andrew P. Witkin,et al.  Scale-Space Filtering , 1983, IJCAI.

[16]  E. Brenner,et al.  A new view on grasping. , 1999, Motor control.

[17]  H. Bülthoff,et al.  Separate neural pathways for the visual analysis of object shape in perception and prehension , 1994, Current Biology.

[18]  P. Baraduc,et al.  Hand orientation for grasping depends on the direction of the reaching movement , 2000, Brain Research.

[19]  Peter Dixon,et al.  Dynamic effects of the Ebbinghaus illusion in grasping: Support for a planning/control model of action , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.

[20]  G P Bingham,et al.  Distortions in definite distance and shape perception as measured by reaching without and with haptic feedback. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Influence of object position and size on human prehension movements , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  M. Jeannerod The neural and behavioural organization of goal-directed movements , 1990, Psychological Medicine.

[23]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Orienting the finger opposition space during prehension movements. , 1994, Journal of motor behavior.