The dominant and nondominant arms are specialized for stabilizing different features of task performance

We have previously proposed a model of motor lateralization, in which the two arms are differentially specialized for complementary control processes. During aimed movements, the dominant arm shows advantages for coordinating intersegmental dynamics as required for specifying trajectory speed and direction, while the nondominant arm shows advantages in controlling limb impedance, as required for accurate final position control. We now directly test this model of lateralization by comparing performance of the two arms under two different tasks: one in which reaching movement is made from one fixed starting position to three different target positions; and the other in which reaching is made from three different starting positions to one fixed target position. For the dominant arm, performance was most accurate when reaching from one fixed starting position to multiple targets. In contrast, nondominant arm performance was most accurate when reaching toward a single target from multiple start locations. These findings contradict the idea that motor lateralization reflects a global advantage of one “dominant” hemisphere/limb system. Instead, each hemisphere/limb system appears specialized for stabilizing different aspects of task performance.

[1]  Robert L Sainburg,et al.  Interlimb differences in control of movement extent. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  J. T. Massey,et al.  Interruption of motor cortical discharge subserving aimed arm movements , 1983, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[4]  P. D. Cheney,et al.  Corticomotoneuronal connections of precentral cells detected by post-spike averages of EMG activity in behaving monkeys , 1976, Brain Research.

[5]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  The Cascade Neural Network Model and a Speed-Accuracy Trade-Off of Arm Movement. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[6]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging of motor cortex: hemispheric asymmetry and handedness. , 1993, Science.

[7]  M. Latash,et al.  Effects of altering initial position on movement direction and extent. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  E. Evarts,et al.  Relation of pyramidal tract activity to force exerted during voluntary movement. , 1968, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  Paul Cisek,et al.  Neural activity in primary motor and dorsal premotor cortex in reaching tasks with the contralateral versus ipsilateral arm. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  Mark L. Latash,et al.  Independent control of joint stiffness in the framework of the equilibrium-point hypothesis , 1993, Biological Cybernetics.

[11]  D. Hoffman,et al.  Direction of action is represented in the ventral premotor cortex , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[12]  Robert L Sainburg,et al.  Nondominant arm advantages in load compensation during rapid elbow joint movements. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Comparison of variability of initial kinematics and endpoints of reaching movements , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  Paul DiZio,et al.  Adaptation in a rotating artificial gravity environment , 1998, Brain Research Reviews.

[15]  R L Sainburg,et al.  Intersegmental dynamics are controlled by sequential anticipatory, error correction, and postural mechanisms. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  R. Sainburg,et al.  Interlimb transfer of visuomotor rotations: independence of direction and final position information , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[17]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Covariation of primate dorsal premotor cell activity with direction and amplitude during a memorized-delay reaching task. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  Jinsung Wang,et al.  Interlimb transfer of novel inertial dynamics is asymmetrical. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  N. Geschwind The apraxias: neural mechanisms of disorders of learned movement. , 1975, American scientist.

[20]  E Donchin,et al.  Studies of Squeezing: Handedness, Responding Hand, Response Force, and Asymmetry of Readiness Potential , 1974, Science.

[21]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[22]  H. Liepmann,et al.  Die linke Hemisphäre und das Handeln.1) , 1908 .

[23]  J. Gordon,et al.  Accuracy of planar reaching movements , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  Slobodan Jaric,et al.  The effects of practice on movement distance and final position reproduction: implications for the equilibrium-point control of movements , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  Robert L. Sainburg,et al.  optimization through lateralization: the evolution of handedness , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[26]  J. Kelso,et al.  Exploring a vibratory systems analysis of human movement production. , 1980, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  Jinsung Wang,et al.  Adaptation to Visuomotor Rotations Remaps Movement Vectors, Not Final Positions , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  An internal model for sensorimotor integration. , 1995, Science.

[29]  D. Hoffman,et al.  Muscle and movement representations in the primary motor cortex. , 1999, Science.

[30]  P. Viviani,et al.  Pointing errors reflect biases in the perception of the initial hand position. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  R. Sainburg Evidence for a dynamic-dominance hypothesis of handedness , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  Robert L Sainburg,et al.  Handedness: dominant arm advantages in control of limb dynamics. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Processes controlling arm movements in monkeys. , 1978, Science.

[34]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning , 1999, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[35]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Arm trajectory formation in monkeys , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[36]  T. Ebner,et al.  Neuronal specification of direction and distance during reaching movements in the superior precentral premotor area and primary motor cortex of monkeys. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  G. Vallortigara,et al.  survival with an asymmetrical brain: advantages and disadvantages of cerebral lateralization , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[38]  R. Sainburg,et al.  Differences in control of limb dynamics during dominant and nondominant arm reaching. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  K. Kurata,et al.  Premotor cortex of monkeys: set- and movement-related activity reflecting amplitude and direction of wrist movements. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  Gregor Thut,et al.  Intermanual transfer of proximal and distal motor engrams in humans , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[41]  G L Gottlieb,et al.  On the voluntary movement of compliant (inertial-viscoelastic) loads by parcellated control mechanisms. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Computational nature of human adaptive control during learning of reaching movements in force fields , 1999, Biological Cybernetics.

[43]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Primate motor cortex and free arm movements to visual targets in three- dimensional space. III. Positional gradients and population coding of movement direction from various movement origins , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[44]  R A Scheidt,et al.  Persistence of motor adaptation during constrained, multi-joint, arm movements. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[45]  William W. Ireland,et al.  On the Left Hemisphere and Motor Actions [Die Linke-Hemisphäre und das Handeln] [original articles in the Münch med. Wochenschrift, Nos. 48 and 49]. (Cbl. für Nervenheilkunde u. Psychiat., July 15th, 1906.) Liepmann , 1907 .

[46]  Robert L. Sainburg,et al.  Lateralization of motor adaptation reveals independence in control of trajectory and steady-state position , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[47]  D M Wolpert,et al.  Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control , 1998, Neural Networks.

[48]  Robert L. Sainburg,et al.  Interlimb transfer of visuomotor rotations depends on handedness , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.