A lower visual field advantage for endpoint stability but no advantage for online movement precision

It has been proposed that visually guided reaching movements performed in the lower visual field (LVF) of peripersonal space are more effective and efficient than their upper visual field (UVF) counterparts (Danckert and Goodale 2001). In the present investigation we sought to determine whether this purported visual field asymmetry reflects advantaged processing of online visual feedback. To accomplish that objective, participants performed discrete reaching movements to each of three target locations in the LVF and UVF. In addition, reaches were completed under conditions wherein target location remained constant throughout a reaching response (i.e., control trials) and a separate condition wherein target location unexpectedly perturbed at movement onset (i.e., experimental trials). We reasoned that the target perturbation paradigm would provide a novel means to assess a possible superior–inferior visual field asymmetry for online reaching control. In terms of the impact of a target perturbation, both visual fields demonstrated equal proficiency integrating visual feedback for online limb adjustments. Interestingly, however, the spatial distribution of movement endpoints in the LVF was less than UVF counterparts (cf. Binsted and Heath 2005). Taken together, the present findings suggest that although LVF and UVF reaches readily use visual feedback to accommodate an unexpected target perturbation, reaches in the LVF elicit advantaged spatial benefits influencing the effectiveness of online limb corrections.

[1]  Robert Sessions Woodworth,et al.  THE ACCURACY OF VOLUNTARY MOVEMENT , 1899 .

[2]  M A VINCE,et al.  Corrective Movements in a Pursuit Task 1 , 1948, Quarterly journal of experimental physiology and cognate medical sciences.

[3]  P. Fitts,et al.  The learning of sequential dependencies. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.

[4]  P. Fitts The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.

[5]  R. L. Deininger,et al.  S-R compatibility: correspondence among paired elements within stimulus and response codes. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.

[6]  Richard Held,et al.  Technique for Studying Adaptation to Disarranged Hand-Eye Coordination , 1958 .

[7]  Steven W. Keele,et al.  Movement control in skilled motor performance. , 1968 .

[8]  W D Beggs,et al.  The accuracy of aiming at a target. Some further evidence for a theory of intermittent control. , 1972, Acta psychologica.

[9]  V. Mountcastle,et al.  Posterior parietal association cortex of the monkey: command functions for operations within extrapersonal space. , 1975, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  D. Stubbs What the eye tells the hand. , 1976, Journal of motor behavior.

[11]  D. Chaffin,et al.  An investigation of fitts' law using a wide range of movement amplitudes. , 1976, Journal of motor behavior.

[12]  M. Bryden Measuring handedness with questionnaires , 1977, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  Les G. Carlton,et al.  Visual Information: The Control of Aiming Movements , 1981 .

[14]  John F. Kalaska,et al.  Spatial coding of movement: A hypothesis concerning the coding of movement direction by motor cortical populations , 1983 .

[15]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement , 1986, Nature.

[16]  C. MacKenzie,et al.  Three-Dimensional Movement Trajectories in Fitts' Task: Implications for Control , 1987 .

[17]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Topographic organization of the middle temporal visual area in the macaque monkey: Representational biases and the relationship to callosal connections and myeloarchitectonic boundaries , 1987, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[18]  R A Abrams,et al.  Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. , 1988, Psychological review.

[19]  E. Hoffmann,et al.  Geometrical conditions for ballistic and visually controlled movements. , 1988, Ergonomics.

[20]  F. Previc Functional specialization in the lower and upper visual fields in humans: Its ecological origins and neurophysiological implications , 1990, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[21]  C. Curcio,et al.  Topography of ganglion cells in human retina , 1990, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[22]  W A MacKay,et al.  Properties of reach-related neuronal activity in cortical area 7A. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  M. Goodale,et al.  Grasping versus pointing and the differential use of visual feedback , 1993 .

[24]  D. Elliott,et al.  Visual regulation of manual aiming , 1993 .

[25]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Influence of object position and size on human prehension movements , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  Romeo Chua,et al.  On-line control of rapid aiming movements: Unexpected target perturbations and movement kinematics. , 1998 .

[27]  F. Previc The neuropsychology of 3-D space. , 1998, Psychological bulletin.

[28]  Matthew Heath,et al.  Visual-Motor Integration of Unexpected Sensory Events in Young and Older Participants: A Kinematic Analysis , 1999 .

[29]  Romeo Chua,et al.  Goal-Directed Aiming: Correcting a Force-Specification Error With the Right and Left Hands. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.

[30]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Role of the posterior parietal cortex in updating reaching movements to a visual target , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  J. Jonas,et al.  Retinal nerve fiber layer thickness in human eyes , 1999, Graefe's Archive for Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology.

[32]  Matthew Heath,et al.  The control of goal-directed limb movements: Correcting errors in the trajectory , 1999 .

[33]  D. Willingham,et al.  Effect of Sex and Joystick Experience on Pursuit Tracking in Adults , 2000, Journal of motor behavior.

[34]  A. Berthoz,et al.  The neural basis of egocentric and allocentric coding of space in humans: a functional magnetic resonance study , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[35]  M. Desmurget,et al.  An ‘automatic pilot’ for the hand in human posterior parietal cortex: toward reinterpreting optic ataxia , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[36]  W. Helsen,et al.  A century later: Woodworth's (1899) two-component model of goal-directed aiming. , 2001, Psychological bulletin.

[37]  M. Goodale,et al.  Superior performance for visually guided pointing in the lower visual field , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  A lesion of the posterior parietal cortex disrupts on-line adjustments during aiming movements , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  James L. Lyons,et al.  Optimal Control Strategies Under Different Feedback Schedules: Kinematic Evidence , 2002, Journal of motor behavior.

[40]  Scott H. Johnson-Frey Taking action : cognitive neuroscience perspectives on intentional acts , 2003 .

[41]  Matthew Heath,et al.  Can a visual representation support the online control of memory-dependent reaching? Evident from a variable spatial mapping paradigm. , 2003, Motor control.

[42]  Melvyn A. Goodale,et al.  A haptic size-contrast illusion affects size perception but not grasping , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[43]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Graspable objects grab attention when the potential for action is recognized , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[44]  Matthew Heath,et al.  Background visual cues and memory-guided reaching. , 2004, Human movement science.

[45]  P. Bédard,et al.  On-line vs. off-line utilization of peripheral visual afferent information to ensure spatial accuracy of goal-directed movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[46]  J. T. Massey,et al.  Interruption of motor cortical discharge subserving aimed arm movements , 1983, Experimental Brain Research.

[47]  Matthew Heath,et al.  The control of memory-guided reaching movements in peripersonal space. , 2004, Motor control.

[48]  Matthew Heath,et al.  No evidence of a lower visual field specialization for visuomotor control , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[49]  M. Goodale,et al.  Visual control of reaching movements without vision of the limb , 1986, Experimental Brain Research.

[50]  M. Goodale,et al.  Visual control of reaching movements without vision of the limb , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[51]  Louise Lakha,et al.  Lower visual field advantage for motion segmentation during high competition for selection. , 2005, Spatial vision.

[52]  Digby Elliott,et al.  Energy-minimization bias: compensating for intrinsic influence of energy-minimization mechanisms. , 2005, Motor control.

[53]  M. Goodale,et al.  Peripheral vision for perception and action , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[54]  Gavin P. Lawrence,et al.  Differences in visuomotor control between the upper and lower visual fields , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[55]  M. Heath Role of limb and target vision in the online control of memory-guided reaches. , 2005, Motor control.