Cephalopods and the evolution of the mind

IN thinking about the nature of the mind and its evolutionary history, cephalopods — especially octopuses, cuttlefish, and squid — have a special importance. These animals are an independent experiment in the evolution of large and complex nervous systems — in the biological machinery of the mind. They evolved this machinery on a historical lineage distant from our own. Where their minds differ from ours, they show us another way of being a sentient organism. Where we are similar, this is due to the convergence of distinct evolutionary paths. I introduced the topic just now as ‘the mind.’ This is a contentious term to use. What is it to have a mind? One option is that we are looking for something close to what humans have — something like reflective and conscious thought. This sets a high bar for having a mind. Another possible view is that whenever organisms adapt to their circumstances in real time by adjusting their behaviour, taking in information and acting in response to it, there is some degree of mentality or intelligence there. To say this sets a low bar. It is best not to set bars in either place. Roughly speaking, we are dealing with a matter of degree, though ‘degree’ is not quite the right term either. The evolution of a mind is the acquisition of a tool-kit for the control of behaviour. The tool-kit includes some kind of perception, though different animals have very different ways of taking in information from the world. It includes some form of memory and learning, means by which past experiences can be brought to bear on the present. In some cases it includes problem-solving and planning. Some tool-kits are more elaborate and expensive than others, but they can be sophisticated in different ways, with different tools present and more investment in one technology than another. One animal might have better ways of tracking the environment through its senses, while another may have simpler senses but more sophisticated learning. Different tool kits go with different ways of making a living. The ordinary term ‘mind’ is awkwardly or misleadingly applied to an animal with a very simple behavioural repertoire, but it is parochial to apply it only to humans.

[1]  B. Baars A cognitive theory of consciousness , 1988 .

[2]  G. Fiorito,et al.  Observational Learning in Octopus vulgaris , 1992, Science.

[3]  T. Cronin,et al.  Learning in Stomatopod Crustaceans , 2006 .

[4]  N. Block,et al.  The nature of consciousness : philosophical debates , 1997 .

[5]  B Mazzolai,et al.  Design of a biomimetic robotic octopus arm , 2009, Bioinspiration & biomimetics.

[6]  Richard W. Byrne,et al.  Sociality, Evolution and Cognition , 2007, Current Biology.

[7]  John Zachary Young,et al.  The Brains and Lives of Cephalopods , 2003 .

[8]  N. Humphrey The Social Function of Intellect , 1976 .

[9]  Peter Carruthers,et al.  An architecture for dual reasoning , 2006 .

[10]  Binyamin Hochner,et al.  A new perspective on the organization of an invertebrate brain. , 2011, Communicative & integrative biology.

[11]  A. Jolly,et al.  Lemur Social Behavior and Primate Intelligence , 1966, Science.

[12]  T. Tregenza,et al.  Female impersonation as an alternative reproductive strategy in giant cuttlefish , 1999, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[13]  T. Tregenza,et al.  Defensive tool use in a coconut-carrying octopus , 2009, Current Biology.

[14]  A. Packard,et al.  CEPHALOPODS AND FISH: THE LIMITS OF CONVERGENCE , 1972 .

[15]  J. Boal Social recognition : A top down view of cephalopod behaviour , 2006 .

[16]  R. Chichery The behavior and natural history of the Caribbean reef squid sepioteuthis sepioidea M. Moynihan and A.F. Rodaniche. (Advances in Ethology, No. 25). Parey, Berlin-Hamburg, 1982, 151 pp., DM 48, ISBN 3-489-61936-6. , 1985, Behavioural Processes.

[17]  B. Hochner,et al.  The Octopus: A Model for a Comparative Analysis of the Evolution of Learning and Memory Mechanisms , 2006, The Biological Bulletin.

[18]  Christophe Geiger,et al.  The Social Function of Intellectual Property Rights, or How Ethics can Influence the Shape and Use of IP Law , 2013 .

[19]  R. Hinde,et al.  Growing Points in Ethology , 1976 .

[20]  R A Brooks,et al.  New Approaches to Robotics , 1991, Science.

[21]  P. Godfrey‐Smith,et al.  Long-term high-density occupation of a site by Octopus tetricus and possible site modification due to foraging behavior , 2012 .

[22]  F. Grasso,et al.  The Evolution of Flexible Behavioral Repertoires in Cephalopod Molluscs , 2009, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.

[23]  R. Hanlon,et al.  Principal features of the mating system of a large spawning aggregation of the giant Australian cuttlefish Sepiaapama (Mollusca: Cephalopoda) , 2002 .

[24]  L. Moroz On the Independent Origins of Complex Brains and Neurons , 2009, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.

[25]  D. Cannon Out of Our Heads: Why You Are Not Your Brain, and Other Lessons from the Biology of Consciousness , 2010 .

[26]  David Q. Matus,et al.  Broad phylogenomic sampling improves resolution of the animal tree of life , 2008, Nature.

[27]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework , 2001, Cognition.

[28]  Christof Koch,et al.  A Theory of Consciousness , 2009 .

[29]  Johanna B. Holm,et al.  Octopuses (Octopus bimaculoides) and cuttlefishes (Sepia pharaonis, S. officinalis) can conditionally discriminate , 2007, Animal Cognition.

[30]  Phyllis C. Lee,et al.  Primate Ontogeny, Cognition and Social Behaviour , 1986 .

[31]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Memory and the Computational Brain , 2009 .

[32]  David B. Edelman,et al.  Animal consciousness: a synthetic approach , 2009, Trends in Neurosciences.

[33]  A. Clark Being There: Putting Brain, Body, and World Together Again , 1996 .

[34]  E. Alleva,et al.  Octopus: the ocean’s intelligent invertebrate , 2011 .

[35]  Jennifer A. Mather,et al.  Navigation by spatial memory and use of visual landmarks in octopuses , 1991, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[36]  R. Caldwell,et al.  Mating behavior of Abdopus aculeatus (d’Orbigny 1834) (Cephalopoda: Octopodidae) in the wild , 2008 .

[37]  Jennifer A. Mather,et al.  Cephalopod consciousness: Behavioural evidence , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.

[38]  T. Nagel Mortal Questions: What is it like to be a bat? , 2012 .

[39]  R. Pfeifer,et al.  Self-Organization, Embodiment, and Biologically Inspired Robotics , 2007, Science.

[40]  G. Fiorito,et al.  I Know My Neighbour: Individual Recognition in Octopus vulgaris , 2011, PloS one.

[41]  M. Martindale,et al.  Acoel development supports a simple planula-like urbilaterian , 2008, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[42]  T. Shallice A theory of consciousness. , 1979, Science.

[43]  Jonathan Evans,et al.  In two minds: Dual processes and beyond. , 2009 .

[44]  J. Mather,et al.  Octopuses (Enteroctopus dofleini) Recognize Individual Humans , 2010, Journal of applied animal welfare science : JAAWS.

[45]  B. Hochner,et al.  Octopus vulgaris Uses Visual Information to Determine the Location of Its Arm , 2011, Current Biology.

[46]  V. Braithwaite,et al.  Pain perception, aversion and fear in fish. , 2007, Diseases of aquatic organisms.

[47]  J. Mather,et al.  Octopus: The Ocean's Intelligent Invertebrate , 2010 .

[48]  Olaf Breidbach,et al.  The Nervous Systems of Invertebrates: An Evolutionary and Comparative Approach , 1995, Experientia Supplementum.

[49]  B. Budelmann The cephalopod nervous system: What evolution has made of the molluscan design , 1995 .

[50]  A. Packard The Behavior and Natural History of the Caribbean Reef Squid (Sepioteuthis sepioidea), Martin Moynihan, Arcadio F. Rodaniche. Paul Parey, Hamburg (1982), 152, Price $21.60 (paperback) , 1983 .

[51]  Michael M. Halassa,et al.  Integrated brain circuits: astrocytic networks modulate neuronal activity and behavior. , 2010, Annual review of physiology.

[52]  HighWire Press Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London , 1781, The London Medical Journal.