Early extrastriate activity without primary visual cortex in humans

Damage to the primary visual cortex (V1) destroys the major source of anatomical input to extrastriate cortical areas (V2, V3, V4 and V5) and produces cortical blindness--an absence of any sensation of light and colour--in the visual field contralateral to the side of the lesion. Neuroimaging studies, nevertheless, have recently demonstrated dorsal and ventral extrastriate activation for stationary stimuli presented to the blind visual field in the absence of V1 activity in human subjects. To clarify the moment in time that visual information reaches extrastriate areas, by means of event-related potentials (ERPs) we tracked the temporal course of responses to complex visual stimuli (faces) presented in the blind field of a hemianopic patient. Stimulation of the normal visual field elicited a positive occipital deflection (P1) at 140 ms. A P1 response was also observed with stimulation of the blind field, although slightly delayed (20 ms) and reduced. Its topography and timing demonstrate that early neural activity for stationary stimuli takes place within extrastriate regions despite V1 denervation.

[1]  K. H. Ruddock,et al.  Visual discrimination of target displacement remains after damage to the striate cortex in humans , 1986, Nature.

[2]  A. Marcel,et al.  Blindsight and shape perception: deficit of visual consciousness or of visual function? , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[3]  D. Milner Insights into blindsight , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[4]  R. Held,et al.  Residual Visual Function after Brain Wounds involving the Central Visual Pathways in Man , 1973, Nature.

[5]  B. Wandell,et al.  Topographic Organization of Human Visual Areas in the Absence of Input from Primary Cortex , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  C. Kennard,et al.  Residual colour vision in a human hemianope: spectral responses and colour discrimination , 1994, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[7]  L Weiskrantz,et al.  Visual capacity in the hemianopic field following a restricted occipital ablation. , 1974, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[8]  R. Malach,et al.  Object-related activity revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging in human occipital cortex. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[9]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Involvement of striate and extrastriate visual cortical areas in spatial attention , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[10]  K H Ruddock,et al.  Human visual responses in the absence of the geniculo-calcarine projection. , 1980, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  L. Weiskrantz,et al.  Parameters affecting conscious versus unconscious visual discrimination with damage to the visual cortex (V1). , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[12]  A. Kleinschmidt,et al.  No visual responses in denervated V1: high‐resolution functional magnetic resonance imaging of a blindsight patient , 1998, Neuroreport.

[13]  S Zeki,et al.  Conscious visual perception without V1. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[14]  K H Ruddock,et al.  Residual vision in patients with retrogeniculate lesions of the visual pathways. , 1987, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[15]  P. Stoerig,et al.  Chromaticity and achromaticity. Evidence for a functional differentiation in visual field defects. , 1987, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[16]  A. Cowey,et al.  Wavelength discrimination in blindsight. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[17]  L Weiskrantz,et al.  Factors affecting visual sensitivity in a hemianopic subject. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.