Effect of Age, Sex, Stimulus Intensity, and Eccentricity on Saccadic Reaction Time in Eye Movement Perimetry

Purpose In eye movement perimetry (EMP), the extent of the visual field is tested by assessing the saccades using an eye tracker. The aim of the present study was to determine the effects of age and sex of the subjects, the eccentricity and intensity of the peripheral stimuli on saccadic reaction time (SRT), and the interaction between these parameters in healthy participants. Methods Healthy participants aged between 20 to 70 years underwent a complete ophthalmic examination and an EMP test. SRT was determined from detected peripheral stimuli of four intensity levels. A multilevel mixed-model analysis was used to verify the influence of subject and stimulus characteristics on SRT within the tested visual field. Results Ninety-five subjects (mean age 43.0 [15.0] years) were included. Age, stimulus intensity, and eccentricity had a statistically significant effect on SRT, not sex. SRTs were significantly faster with increasing stimulus intensity and decreasing eccentricity (P < 0.001). At the lowest stimulus intensity of 192 cd/m2, a significant interaction was found between age and eccentricity. Conclusions The current study demonstrated significant SRT dependence across the visual field measured up to 27°, irrespective of sex. The presented SRT values may serve as a first normative guide for EMP. Translational Relevance This report of SRT interaction can aid in refining its use as a measure of visual field responsiveness.

[1]  Ben Kenward,et al.  Saccadic Reaction Times in Infants and Adults: Spatiotemporal Factors, Gender, and Interlaboratory Variation , 2017, Developmental psychology.

[2]  H. Lemij,et al.  Development of a test grid using Eye Movement Perimetry for screening glaucomatous visual field defects , 2017, Graefe's Archive for Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology.

[3]  J. Fuller,et al.  Eye position and target amplitude effects on human visual saccadic latencies , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  M. Eizenman,et al.  Eye movement perimetry , 1995, Proceedings of 17th International Conference of the Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society.

[5]  A T Bahill,et al.  Types of saccadic eye movements , 1979, Neurology.

[6]  Usha Chakravarthy,et al.  Prevalence of Cataract in an Older Population in India , 2011, Ophthalmology.

[7]  Douglas R. Anderson Automated Static Perimetry , 1992 .

[8]  A. Opstal,et al.  Stimulus intensity modifies saccadic reaction time and visual response latency in the superior colliculus , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  Burkhart Fischer,et al.  On the development of voluntary and reflexive components in human saccade generation , 1997, Brain Research.

[10]  H. Brash,et al.  Feasibility of saccadic vector optokinetic perimetry: a method of automated static perimetry for children using eye tracking. , 2009, Ophthalmology.

[11]  Hans Limburg,et al.  Global causes of blindness and distance vision impairment 1990-2020: a systematic review and meta-analysis. , 2017, The Lancet. Global health.

[12]  Ronnie George,et al.  Comparison of saccadic reaction time between normal and glaucoma using an eye movement perimeter , 2014, Indian journal of ophthalmology.

[13]  Elizabeth L Irving,et al.  Horizontal saccade dynamics across the human life span. , 2006, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[14]  Andrew J. Tatham,et al.  Comparison of Threshold Saccadic Vector Optokinetic Perimetry (SVOP) and Standard Automated Perimetry (SAP) in Glaucoma. Part II: Patterns of Visual Field Loss and Acceptability , 2017, Translational vision science & technology.

[15]  Stephen F. Barnes The Aging Human Brain , 2010 .

[16]  R. Raman,et al.  Retinal sensitivity in healthy Indians using microperimeter , 2014, Indian journal of ophthalmology.

[17]  David R Pepperberg,et al.  Bleaching desensitization: background and current challenges , 2003, Vision Research.

[18]  J. Steen,et al.  Assessment of visual orienting behaviour in young children using remote eye tracking: Methodology and reliability , 2010, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[19]  D. Munoz,et al.  The influence of stimulus direction and eccentricity on pro- and anti-saccades in humans , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  David E. Warren,et al.  Perimetric evaluation of saccadic latency, saccadic accuracy, and visual threshold for peripheral visual stimuli in young compared with older adults. , 2013, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[21]  J. Pratt,et al.  Initiation and inhibition of saccadic eye movements in younger and older adults: an analysis of the gap effect. , 1997, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[22]  N. Shimizu [Neurology of eye movements]. , 2000, Rinsho shinkeigaku = Clinical neurology.

[23]  Ómar I. Jóhannesson,et al.  Saccade performance in the nasal and temporal hemifields , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  J. R. Rosenberg,et al.  An analysis of the dependence of saccadic latency on target position and target characteristics in human subjects , 2001, BMC Neuroscience.

[25]  J. Barbur,et al.  Exploring Eye Movements in Patients with Glaucoma When Viewing a Driving Scene , 2010, PloS one.

[26]  Lance M. Optican,et al.  Superior colliculus cell types and models of saccade generation , 1994, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[27]  Gijs Thepass,et al.  Validity and Repeatability of Saccadic Response Times Across the Visual Field in Eye Movement Perimetry. , 2013, Translational vision science & technology.

[28]  Andrew J. Tatham,et al.  Comparison of Saccadic Vector Optokinetic Perimetry and Standard Automated Perimetry in Glaucoma. Part I: Threshold Values and Repeatability , 2017, Translational vision science & technology.

[29]  M. Saslow Effects of components of displacement-step stimuli upon latency for saccadic eye movement. , 1967, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[30]  Jérome Fleuriet,et al.  Saccadic Interception of a Moving Visual Target after a Spatiotemporal Perturbation , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  D. Munoz,et al.  Age-related performance of human subjects on saccadic eye movement tasks , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  Neeru Gupta,et al.  Delayed saccadic eye movements in glaucoma. , 2012, Eye and brain.

[33]  Dan Milea,et al.  Impaired Saccadic Eye Movement in Primary Open-angle Glaucoma , 2014, Journal of glaucoma.

[34]  K. Fukushima,et al.  Development of voluntary control of saccadic eye movements I. Age-related changes in normal children , 2000, Brain and Development.

[35]  PremNandhini Satgunam,et al.  Pediatric Perimeter—A Novel Device to Measure Visual Fields in Infants and Patients with Special Needs , 2017, Translational vision science & technology.

[36]  Ronnie George,et al.  Glaucoma in India: Estimated Burden of Disease , 2010, Journal of glaucoma.

[37]  T. Hodgson,et al.  The location marker effect. Saccadic latency increases with target eccentricity. , 2002, Experimental brain research.

[38]  A. Cowey,et al.  Nasal and temporal retinal ganglion cells projecting to the midbrain: Implications for “blindsight” , 1995, Neuroscience.

[39]  D. Crabb,et al.  Eye movements during visual search in patients with glaucoma , 2012, BMC Ophthalmology.

[40]  H. Lemij,et al.  The Effect of Cataract on Eye Movement Perimetry , 2015, Journal of ophthalmology.

[41]  R. Carpenter,et al.  Contrast, Probability, and Saccadic Latency Evidence for Independence of Detection and Decision , 2004, Current Biology.

[42]  David P Crabb,et al.  Does Glaucoma Alter Eye Movements When Viewing Images of Natural Scenes? A Between-Eye Study. , 2018, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[43]  Qing Yang,et al.  The latency of saccades, vergence, and combined eye movements in children and in adults. , 2002, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.