Lower left temporal-frontal connectivity characterizes expert and accurate performance: High-alpha T7-Fz connectivity as a marker of conscious processing during movement

The Theory of Reinvestment argues that conscious processing can impair motor performance. The present study tested the utility of left temporal-frontal cortical connectivity as a neurophysiological marker of movement specific conscious processing. Expert and novice golfers completed putts while temporal-frontal connectivity was computed using high alpha Inter Site Phase Clustering (ISPC) and then analyzed as a function of experience (experts versus novices), performance (holed versus missed putts), and pressure (low versus high). Existing evidence shows that left temporal to frontal connectivity is related to dispositional conscious processing and is sensitive to the amount of declarative knowledge acquired during learning. We found that T7-Fz ISPC, but not T8-Fz ISPC, was lower in experts than novices, and lower when putts were holed than missed. Accordingly, our findings provide additional evidence that communication between verbal/language and motor areas of the brain during preparation for action and its execution is associated with poor motor performance. Our findings validate high-alpha left temporal-frontal connectivity as a neurophysiological correlate of movement specific conscious processing

[1]  N RichardS.MastersandJamieM.PooLtO,et al.  Advances in implicit motor learning , 2012 .

[2]  D. Goodman,et al.  Cognitive Demands of Error Processing , 2008, Psychological reports.

[3]  Cognitive demands of error processing associated with preparation and execution of a motor skill , 2010, Consciousness and Cognition.

[4]  F. Varela,et al.  Measuring phase synchrony in brain signals , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[5]  Jeremy C. Rietschel,et al.  The influence of social evaluation on cerebral cortical activity and motor performance: a study of "Real-Life" competition. , 2013, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[6]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[7]  Michael X Cohen,et al.  Analyzing Neural Time Series Data: Theory and Practice , 2014 .

[8]  Jeremy C. Rietschel,et al.  Cerebral cortical activity associated with non-experts' most accurate motor performance. , 2014, Human movement science.

[9]  R. Masters,et al.  Investigating the efficacy of neurofeedback training for expediting expertise and excellence in sport , 2015 .

[10]  Robert J. Crutcher,et al.  The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. , 1993 .

[11]  C. Hillman,et al.  Cortico-cortical communication and superior performance in skilled marksmen: an EEG coherence analysis. , 2003 .

[12]  Alexa Riehle,et al.  Preparation for Action: one of the Key Functions of Motor Cortex , 2004 .

[13]  J. Maxwell,et al.  Neural co-activation as a yardstick of implicit motor learning and the propensity for conscious control of movement , 2011, Biological Psychology.

[14]  M. Iacoboni,et al.  “Neural efficiency” of experts’ brain during judgment of actions: A high-resolution EEG study in elite and amateur karate athletes , 2010, Behavioural Brain Research.

[15]  Alexandra T Basford,et al.  Cortical control of motor sequences , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[16]  Jacob Cohen,et al.  A power primer. , 1992, Psychological bulletin.

[17]  M. Iacoboni,et al.  Golf putt outcomes are predicted by sensorimotor cerebral EEG rhythms , 2008, The Journal of physiology.

[18]  E. Oja,et al.  Independent Component Analysis , 2013 .

[19]  A. Cooke Readying the head and steadying the heart: a review of cortical and cardiac studies of preparation for action in sport , 2013 .

[20]  R. Baumeister,et al.  A review of paradoxical performance effects: Choking under pressure in sports and mental tests , 1986 .

[21]  Steven W. Keele,et al.  Movement control in skilled motor performance. , 1968 .

[22]  R. Masters Knowledge, knerves and know-how: The role of explicit versus implicit knowledge in the breakdown of a complex motor skill under pressure , 1992 .

[23]  A. Mognon,et al.  ADJUST: An automatic EEG artifact detector based on the joint use of spatial and temporal features. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[24]  Mark R. Wilson,et al.  Quiet eye training: A means to implicit motor learning , 2013 .

[25]  H. Jasper,et al.  The ten-twenty electrode system of the International Federation. The International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. , 1999, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.

[26]  P. Rossini,et al.  Intra-hemispheric functional coupling of alpha rhythms is related to golfer's performance: a coherence EEG study. , 2011, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[27]  David McIntyre,et al.  Effects of competitive pressure on expert performance: underlying psychological, physiological, and kinematic mechanisms. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[28]  Rich S. W. Masters,et al.  The theory of reinvestment , 2008 .

[29]  Adrian R. Willoughby,et al.  Premovement high‐alpha power is modulated by previous movement errors: Indirect evidence to endorse high‐alpha power as a marker of resource allocation during motor programming , 2015, Psychophysiology.

[30]  J. Schoffelen,et al.  Source connectivity analysis with MEG and EEG , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[31]  Bradley D. Hatfield,et al.  Electroencephalographic Coherence During Visuomotor Performance: A Comparison of Cortico-Cortical Communication in Experts and Novices , 2009, Journal of motor behavior.

[32]  R. Ryan,et al.  Control and information in the intrapersonal sphere: An extension of cognitive evaluation theory. , 1982 .

[33]  Tsung-Min Hung,et al.  Electroencephalographic Studies of Skilled Psychomotor Performance , 2004, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[34]  A. Williams,et al.  Skill acquisition in sport: Research, theory and practice , 2005, Sport & Exercise Psychology Review.

[35]  Germano Gallicchio,et al.  Preparation for action: Psychophysiological activity preceding a motor skill as a function of expertise, performance outcome, and psychological pressure , 2014, Psychophysiology.

[36]  P. Welch The use of fast Fourier transform for the estimation of power spectra: A method based on time averaging over short, modified periodograms , 1967 .

[37]  W. Klimesch Alpha-band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored information , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[38]  R. Masters,et al.  Reinvestment and Movement Disruption Following Stroke , 2009, Neurorehabilitation and neural repair.

[39]  Tzyy-Ping Jung,et al.  Independent Component Analysis of Electroencephalographic Data , 1995, NIPS.