Comparison of Brain Activation in Response to Two Dimensional and Three Dimensional On-Line Games

Objective The present study assessed the difference in the brain activity of professional gamers (excessive players, but not addicts) in response to playing a 3-dimensional online game with an improved interface. Methods Twenty-three StarCraft I pro gamers and 16 StarCraft II pro gamers were recruited at Chung Ang University Medical Center. Brain activity in response to StarCraft I or II cues was assessed with a 1.5 Tesla Espree MRI scanner. Results StarCraft I pro gamers showed significantly greater activity in 4 clusters in response to the video game cues compared to StarCraft II pro gamers: right superior frontal gyrus, right medial frontal gyrus, right occipital lobe, and left medial frontal gyrus. StarCraft II pro gamers showed significantly greater activity in 3 clusters in response to the video game cues compared to StarCraft I pro gamers: left middle frontal gyrus, left temporal fusiform gyrus and left cerebellum. Discussion This is the first study to show the difference in brain activity between gamers playing either a 2-dimensional or 3-dimensional online game. Current brain imaging studies may confirm the pro gamers' experience when playing StarCraft II, a 3-dimensional game with an improved interface, relative to playing StarCraft I.

[1]  D D Hoffman,et al.  Completing visual contours: The relationship between relatability and minimizing inflections , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[2]  Jonathan Rosenhead,et al.  Soft Systems Methodology in Action , 1991 .

[3]  R Vogels,et al.  Macaque inferior temporal neurons are selective for disparity-defined three-dimensional shapes. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[4]  Guy A. Orban,et al.  The Extraction of 3D Shape from Texture and Shading in the Human Brain , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[5]  In Kyoon Lyoo,et al.  Brain activity and desire for Internet video game play. , 2011, Comprehensive psychiatry.

[6]  A. Neubauer,et al.  Two- vs. three-dimensional presentation of mental rotation tasks: Sex differences and effects of training on performance and brain activation , 2010, Intelligence.

[7]  T Nakada,et al.  Perceptual processing of stereopsis in humans: high-field (3.0-tesla) functional MRI study. , 1999, Neurology.

[8]  J. Todd Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.8 No.3 March 2004 The visual perception of 3D shape q , 2022 .

[9]  C. S. Green,et al.  Changes in search rate but not in the dynamics of exogenous attention in action videogame players , 2011, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[10]  G. Orban,et al.  Attention to 3-D Shape, 3-D Motion, and Texture in 3-D Structure from Motion Displays , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[11]  G. Orban,et al.  Three-Dimensional Shape Coding in Inferior Temporal Cortex , 2000, Neuron.

[12]  Xiaolin Zhou,et al.  Distinct Neural Correlates for Resolving Stroop Conflict at Inhibited and Noninhibited Locations in Inhibition of Return , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[13]  Pascal Mamassian,et al.  Prior knowledge of illumination for 3D perception in the human brain , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[14]  James T Todd,et al.  The visual perception of 3-D shape from multiple cues: Are observers capable of perceiving metric structure? , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.

[15]  Erik Bethke,et al.  Game development and production , 2003 .

[16]  G. Orban,et al.  Macaque Inferior Temporal Neurons Are Selective for Three-Dimensional Boundaries and Surfaces , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  T. Robbins,et al.  Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[18]  Brigitte Röder,et al.  Attending to visual or auditory motion affects perception within and across modalities: an event‐related potential study , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[19]  Narumi Katsuyama,et al.  Perception of object motion in three-dimensional space induced by cast shadows , 2011, NeuroImage.

[20]  Masayuki Nakajima,et al.  Study of asthenopia caused by the viewing of stereoscopic images: measurement by MEG and other devices , 2006, Electronic Imaging.

[21]  J J Koenderink,et al.  Direct Measurement of the Curvature of Visual Space , 2000, Perception.

[22]  Takeo Watanabe,et al.  3D surface perception from motion involves a temporal–parietal network , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.

[23]  A. Beck,et al.  An inventory for measuring depression. , 1961, Archives of general psychiatry.

[24]  Ryuta Ito,et al.  Common neural processing regions for dynamic and static stereopsis in human parieto-occipital cortices , 2002, Neuroscience Letters.

[25]  J. Galen Buckwalter,et al.  Sex differences in mental rotation and spatial rotation in a virtual environment , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[26]  A. Kramer,et al.  Videogame training strategy-induced change in brain function during a complex visuomotor task , 2012, Behavioural Brain Research.

[27]  F. Gonzalez,et al.  Evidence of basal temporo-occipital cortex involvement in stereoscopic vision in humans: a study with subdural electrode recordings. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[28]  Karl Magnus Petersson,et al.  The interaction between surface color and color knowledge: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence , 2011, Brain and Cognition.

[29]  Christa Neuper,et al.  Sex differences in human EEG theta oscillations during spatial navigation in virtual reality. , 2011, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[30]  M Jeannerod,et al.  Visual working memory for shape and 3D‐orientation: a PET study , 1997, Neuroreport.

[31]  R. Malach,et al.  When the Brain Loses Its Self: Prefrontal Inactivation during Sensorimotor Processing , 2006, Neuron.

[32]  Paul M Matthews,et al.  The Role of the Posterior Fusiform Gyrus in Reading , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[33]  G. Orban,et al.  Selectivity for 3D shape that reveals distinct areas within macaque inferior temporal cortex. , 2000, Science.

[34]  In Kyoon Lyoo,et al.  Differential regional gray matter volumes in patients with on-line game addiction and professional gamers. , 2012, Journal of psychiatric research.

[35]  D. A. Huffman,et al.  Realizable Configurations of Lines in Pictures of Polyhedrat , 2013 .

[36]  George I. Christopoulos,et al.  Neural Correlates of Value, Risk, and Risk Aversion Contributing to Decision Making under Risk , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  C. Ko,et al.  Brain activities associated with gaming urge of online gaming addiction. , 2009, Journal of psychiatric research.