Perceived Ambiguity About Cancer Prevention Recommendations: Relationship to Perceptions of Cancer Preventability, Risk, and Worry

In this study, we apply the concept of “ambiguity,” as developed in the decision theory literature, to an analysis of potential psychological consequences of uncertainty about cancer prevention recommendations. We used Health Information National Trends Survey (HINTS) 2003 data to examine how perceived ambiguity about cancer prevention recommendations relates to three other cognitive variables known to influence cancer-protective behavior: perceived cancer preventability, perceived cancer risk, and cancer-related worry. Using logistic regression analyses, we tested several predictions derived from a review of literature on the effects of ambiguity perceptions on decision making, cognitions, and emotions. We found perceived ambiguity to have a strong negative relationship with perceived cancer preventability, consistent with “ambiguity aversion”—a pessimistic bias in the interpretation of ambiguity. Cancer worry moderated this relationship; ambiguity aversion increased with higher levels of worry. At the same time, perceived ambiguity was positively related to both perceived cancer risk and cancer worry. Furthermore, perceived risk partially mediated the relationship between perceived ambiguity and worry. These findings suggest that perceived ambiguity about cancer prevention recommendations may have broad and important effects on other health cognitions. We discuss ethical implications of these findings for health communication efforts, and propose a tentative causal model to guide future research.

[1]  R. Sorrentino,et al.  The Uncertain Mind: Individual Differences in Facing the Unknown , 2002 .

[2]  I. Levin,et al.  Relating individual differences in attitude toward ambiguity to risky choices , 2001 .

[3]  M. Andrykowski,et al.  Physical symptoms, distress, and breast cancer risk perceptions in women with benign breast problems. , 1998, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[4]  Rakesh K. Sarin,et al.  Modeling Ambiguity in Decisions Under Uncertainty , 1988 .

[5]  Nils-Eric Sahlin,et al.  Decision making with unreliable probabilities , 1983 .

[6]  E. Emanuel,et al.  Four models of the physician-patient relationship. , 1992, JAMA.

[7]  J G Thornton,et al.  Informed decision making: an annotated bibliography and systematic review. , 1999, Health technology assessment.

[8]  Robert M Kaplan,et al.  Shared medical decision making. A new tool for preventive medicine. , 2004, American journal of preventive medicine.

[9]  K. Warriner,et al.  Evaluating Socio-economic Status (SES) Bias in Survey Nonresponse , 2002 .

[10]  B. Rimer,et al.  Informed decision making: What is its role in cancer screening? , 2004, Cancer.

[11]  W. Viscusi,et al.  Smoking Status and Public Responses to Ambiguous Scientific Risk Evidence , 1999 .

[12]  Gary L. Kreps,et al.  The Health Information National Trends Survey (HINTS): Development, Design, and Dissemination , 2004, Journal of health communication.

[13]  Shelly Chaiken,et al.  Defensive Processing of Personally Relevant Health Messages , 1992 .

[14]  Lisa V John,et al.  To screen or not to screen, when clinical guidelines disagree: primary care physicians' use of the PSA test. , 2004, Preventive medicine.

[15]  S. Taplin,et al.  Conflicting National Recommendations and the Use of Screening Mammography: Does the Physician's Recommendation Matter? , 1997, The Journal of the American Board of Family Medicine.

[16]  J. Schorling,et al.  The impact of informed consent on patient interest in prostate-specific antigen screening. , 1996, Archives of internal medicine.

[17]  M. Castillo,et al.  Mood congruent Bias in Interpretation of Ambiguity Strategic Processes and Temporary Activation , 1997 .

[18]  C MacLeod,et al.  Depression and the interpretation of ambiguity. , 1999, Behaviour research and therapy.

[19]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Recent developments in modeling preferences: Uncertainty and ambiguity , 1992 .

[20]  C. Charles,et al.  Shared decision-making in the medical encounter: what does it mean? (or it takes at least two to tango). , 1997, Social science & medicine.

[21]  A. Raffle,et al.  Information about screening – is it to achieve high uptake or to ensure informed choice? , 2001, Health expectations : an international journal of public participation in health care and health policy.

[22]  J. Schorling,et al.  Predictors of interest in prostate-specific antigen screening and the impact of informed consent: what should we tell our patients? , 1997, The American journal of medicine.

[23]  Marie Helweg-Larsen,et al.  Are Comparative Risk Judgments Consistent Across Time and Events? , 2003, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[24]  S. Fletcher,et al.  Beliefs and expectations of women under 50 years old regarding screening mammography , 2003, Journal of general internal medicine.

[25]  A. Sowden,et al.  Increasing informed uptake and non‐uptake of screening: evidence from a systematic review , 2001, Health expectations : an international journal of public participation in health care and health policy.

[26]  M. Holmes-Rovner,et al.  The mammography screening controversy: who and what is heard in the press? , 2003, Patient education and counseling.

[27]  J. Dietch Anxiety Disorders and Phobias: A Cognitive Perspective , 1986 .

[28]  D. A. Kenny,et al.  The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[29]  R. Fox,et al.  The evolution of medical uncertainty. , 1980, The Milbank Memorial Fund quarterly. Health and society.

[30]  E. D. de Leeuw,et al.  Trends in household survey nonresponse: a longitudinal and international comparison , 2002 .

[31]  J. Lerner,et al.  Fear, anger, and risk. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[32]  R. Hogarth,et al.  Ambiguity and Uncertainty in Probabilistic Inference. , 1985 .

[33]  B. Rimer Interventions to increase breast screening: Lifespan and ethnicity issues , 1994, Cancer.

[34]  C. MacLeod,et al.  Cognitive approaches to emotion and emotional disorders. , 1994, Annual review of psychology.

[35]  R. Myers,et al.  Factors associated with intention to undergo annual prostate cancer screening among African American men in Philadelphia , 1996, Cancer.

[36]  Michael Pignone,et al.  Prostate and colon cancer screening messages in popular magazines , 2004, Journal of General Internal Medicine.

[37]  R. Hogarth,et al.  Decision Making under Ambiguity , 1986 .

[38]  W. Klein,et al.  Breast cancer risk perceptions and breast cancer worry: what predicts what? , 2005 .

[39]  I. Lipkus,et al.  Relationships among breast cancer concern, risk perceptions, and interest in genetic testing for breast cancer susceptibility among African-American women with and without a family history of breast cancer. , 1999, Cancer epidemiology, biomarkers & prevention : a publication of the American Association for Cancer Research, cosponsored by the American Society of Preventive Oncology.

[40]  J. Kassirer,et al.  Clinical research--what should the public believe? , 1994, The New England journal of medicine.

[41]  C. Schneider,et al.  How underlying patient beliefs can affect physician-patient communication about prostate-specific antigen testing. , 2002, Effective clinical practice : ECP.

[42]  Joel Huber,et al.  Communication of ambiguous risk information , 1991 .

[43]  Melissa C. Brouwers,et al.  Uncertainty orientation and protection motivation theory : the role of individual differences in health compliance , 1993 .

[44]  Jonathan Baron,et al.  Ambiguity and rationality , 1988 .

[45]  M. Diefenbach,et al.  Specific worry about breast cancer predicts mammography use in women at risk for breast and ovarian cancer. , 1999, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[46]  Floyd J Fowler,et al.  Enthusiasm for cancer screening in the United States. , 2004, JAMA.

[47]  H. Welch,et al.  Women's understanding of the mammography screening debate. , 2000, Archives of internal medicine.

[48]  B. Rimer,et al.  Knowledge, beliefs, and prior screening behavior among blacks and whites reporting for prostate cancer screening. , 1995, Urology.

[49]  Gideon Keren,et al.  On the robustness and possible accounts of ambiguity aversion , 1999 .

[50]  S. Sonnad,et al.  Mammography messages in popular media: implications for patient expectations and shared clinical decision‐making , 2001, Health expectations : an international journal of public participation in health care and health policy.

[51]  J. Frank Yates,et al.  Characterization of Ambiguity in Decision Making. , 1976 .

[52]  J. Wardle,et al.  Socioeconomic differences in cancer screening participation: comparing cognitive and psychosocial explanations. , 2004, Social science & medicine.

[53]  Baruch Fischhoff,et al.  Treating the public with risk communications: a public health perspective. , 1987, Science, technology & human values.

[54]  C. MacLeod,et al.  Anxiety and the interpretation of ambiguity: a text comprehension study. , 1993, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[55]  J. Austoker Gaining informed consent for screening , 1999, BMJ.

[56]  Shawn P. Curley,et al.  An empirical evaluation of descriptive models of ambiguity reactions in choice situations , 1989 .

[57]  Robert J Volk,et al.  Patient Education for Informed Decision Making About Prostate Cancer Screening: A Randomized Controlled Trial with 1-Year Follow-Up , 2003, The Annals of Family Medicine.

[58]  N. Wald Populist instead of professional , 2000, Journal of Medical Screening.

[59]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  Motivated closing of the mind: "seizing" and "freezing". , 1996, Psychological review.

[60]  D. Ellsberg Decision, probability, and utility: Risk, ambiguity, and the Savage axioms , 1961 .

[61]  H Kunreuther,et al.  Cognitive processes and the decisions of some parents to forego pertussis vaccination for their children. , 1996, Journal of clinical epidemiology.

[62]  Lisa M. Schwartz,et al.  News media coverage of screening mammography for women in their 40s and tamoxifen for primary prevention of breast cancer. , 2002, JAMA.

[63]  Nisha Gandhi,et al.  Promoting informed decisions about cancer screening in communities and healthcare systems. , 2004, American journal of preventive medicine.

[64]  Z. Kunda,et al.  The case for motivated reasoning. , 1990, Psychological bulletin.

[65]  M. Stefanek,et al.  First degree relatives of breast cancer patients: screening practices and provision of risk information. , 1991, Cancer detection and prevention.

[66]  W. Viscusi,et al.  Alarmist Decisions with Divergent Risk Information , 1997 .

[67]  Jonathan Baron,et al.  Behavioral Law and Economics: Reluctance to Vaccinate: Omission Bias and Ambiguity , 1990 .

[68]  R. Abrams,et al.  Psychological sources of ambiguity avoidance , 1986 .

[69]  Shelly Chaiken,et al.  Chronic issue-specific fear inhibits systematic processing of persuasive communications. , 1990 .

[70]  M. Essink‐bot,et al.  Why do men refuse or attend population-based screening for prostate cancer? , 2000, Journal of public health medicine.

[71]  R. Croyle,et al.  Psychological minimization of cholesterol test results: moderators of appraisal in college students and community residents. , 1993, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[72]  G. Yamey,et al.  The PSA storm , 2002, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[73]  T. Borkovec,et al.  Interpretive cues and ambiguity in generalized anxiety disorder. , 2004, Behaviour research and therapy.

[74]  S. Chaiken,et al.  The role of mood and message ambiguity in the interplay of heuristic and systematic processing , 1994 .

[75]  Kathleen N Lohr,et al.  Screening for Prostate Cancer: An Update of the Evidence for the U.S. Preventive Services Task Force , 2002, Annals of Internal Medicine.

[76]  J. Lerner,et al.  Beyond valence: Toward a model of emotion-specific influences on judgement and choice , 2000 .

[77]  A. Tversky,et al.  Preference and belief: Ambiguity and competence in choice under uncertainty , 1991 .

[78]  Robert M. Kaplan,et al.  Evaluation of two methods to facilitate shared decision making for men considering the prostate-specific antigen test , 2001, Journal of General Internal Medicine.

[79]  Adrian Furnham,et al.  Tolerance of ambiguity: A review of the concept, its measurement and applications , 1995 .

[80]  Howard Kunreuther,et al.  Ambiguity and underwriter decision processes , 1995 .

[81]  Howard Leventhal,et al.  The common-sense model of self-regulation of health and illness. , 2003 .

[82]  G. Taubes Epidemiology faces its limits. , 1995, Science.

[83]  S. Whitney A New Model of Medical Decisions: Exploring the Limits of Shared Decision Making , 2003, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[84]  Shawn P. Curley,et al.  The center and range of the probability interval as factors affecting ambiguity preferences , 1985 .