Free and cued selective reminding and selective reminding in the elderly.

Free and Cued Selective Reminding (FCSR) differs from Selective Reminding (SR) because of a study procedure which controls cognitive processing and a reminding procedure which allows for cued recall. Performance on FCSR and SR was compared in two studies to determine which test produces better recall and to identify the factors that account for the superior recall. When the tests were administered to very elderly normal subjects using the standard clinical protocol, twice as many words were retrieved from long-term memory in FCSR than SR. The second experiment, which manipulated study and reminding conditions in a younger sample of normal elderly, suggests that the improvement in free recall was due to the study procedure and the method of reminding.

[1]  W. Nelson Francis,et al.  FREQUENCY ANALYSIS OF ENGLISH USAGE: LEXICON AND GRAMMAR , 1983 .

[2]  E. Tangalos,et al.  Memory function in normal aging , 1992, Neurology.

[3]  H. Buschke,et al.  The effect of dementia risk factors on comparative and diagnostic selective reminding norms , 1997, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[4]  D. Park,et al.  Memory for pictures, words, and spatial location in older adults: evidence for pictorial superiority. , 1983, Journal of gerontology.

[5]  D. Howard,et al.  Category norms: a comparison of the Battig and Montague (1969) norms with the responses of adults between the ages of 20 and 80. , 1980, Journal of gerontology.

[6]  Robert A. Bjork,et al.  A new theory of disuse and an old theory of stimulus fluctuation , 1992 .

[7]  Fergus I. M. Craik,et al.  Aging and Cognitive Deficits , 1982 .

[8]  M. Roth,et al.  The Association Between Quantitative Measures of Dementia and of Senile Change in the Cerebral Grey Matter of Elderly Subjects , 1968, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[9]  H Morgenstern,et al.  Development of dementing illnesses in an 80‐year‐old volunteer cohort , 1989, Annals of neurology.

[10]  Endel Tulving,et al.  Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. , 1973 .

[11]  B. L. Beattie,et al.  Cued recall and early identification of dementia. , 1991, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[12]  E. Tulving,et al.  Amnesia and memory research , 1982 .

[13]  Fergus I. M. Craik,et al.  A processing resource account of age differences in recall. , 1982 .

[14]  Fergus I. M. Craik,et al.  Aging and cognitive deficits : The role of attentional resources , 1982 .

[15]  M. Folstein,et al.  Population-based norms for the Mini-Mental State Examination by age and educational level. , 1993, JAMA.

[16]  M. Sliwinski,et al.  Neuropsychological prediction of dementia and the absence of dementia in healthy elderly persons , 1994, Neurology.

[17]  A. W. Melton The situation with respect to the spacing of repetitions and memory , 1970 .

[18]  W. Whitten Initial-retrieval “depth” and the negative recency effect , 1978 .

[19]  David B. Mitchell,et al.  The Appearance and Disappearance of Age Differences in Adult Memory , 1982 .

[20]  K. Marder,et al.  Neuropsychological detection and characterization of preclinical Alzheimer's disease , 1995, Neurology.

[21]  A. D. Smith The interaction between age and list length in free recall. , 1979, Journal of gerontology.

[22]  H. Tuokko,et al.  Cued recall and memory disorders in dementia. , 1989, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[23]  Robert A. Bjork,et al.  Optimum rehearsal patterns and name learning , 1978 .

[24]  M. Glanzer,et al.  Picture superiority in free recall: the effects of normal aging and primary degenerative dementia. , 1986, Journal of gerontology.

[25]  A. Paivio Mental imagery in associative learning and memory , 1969 .

[26]  H S Levin,et al.  Distinguishing normal and demented elderly with the selective reminding test. , 1989, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[27]  Douglas L. Hintzman,et al.  Theoretical implications of the spacing effect. , 1974 .

[28]  Learning and retention in preclinical and early Alzheimer's disease. , 1997 .

[29]  Herman Buschke,et al.  Selective reminding for analysis of memory and learning , 1973 .

[30]  E G Tangalos,et al.  Memory function in very early Alzheimer's disease , 1994, Neurology.

[31]  W. Montague,et al.  Category norms of verbal items in 56 categories A replication and extension of the Connecticut category norms , 1969 .

[32]  E. Tangalos,et al.  Free and cued selective reminding test: MOANS norms. , 1997, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[33]  F. Craik,et al.  Prior retrieval effects in young and old adults. , 1986, Journal of gerontology.

[34]  H. Buschke,et al.  Implicit and explicit memory in young, old, and demented adults. , 1992, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[35]  A. Paivio,et al.  Picture superiority in free recall: Imagery or dual coding? , 1973 .

[36]  H. Buschke,et al.  Cued recall in amnesia. , 1984, Journal of clinical neuropsychology.

[37]  F. Craik,et al.  Levels of Pro-cessing: A Framework for Memory Research , 1975 .

[38]  R. Mayeux,et al.  Diagnosis of dementia in a heterogeneous population. Development of a neuropsychological paradigm-based diagnosis of dementia and quantified correction for the effects of education. , 1992, Archives of neurology.

[39]  H. Buschke,et al.  Screening for dementia by memory testing , 1988, Neurology.

[40]  H. Buschke,et al.  Aging, encoding specificity, and memory change in the Double Memory Test , 1995, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[41]  M. Perlmutter What Is Memory Aging the Aging Of , 1978 .

[42]  H. Buschke,et al.  Genuine memory deficits in dementia , 1987 .