Do typically and atypically developing children learn and generalize novel names similarly: the role of conceptual distance during learning and at test

There is a large body of evidence showing that comparison of multiple stimuli leads to better conceptualization and generalization of novel names than no-comparison settings in typically developing (TD) children. By contrast, the evidence regarding this issue remains scarce in children with intellectual disabilities (ID). Children with intellectual disabilities (ID) and TD children matched on mental age with the Raven's coloured progressive matrices were tested in several novel name learning comparison conditions, with familiar objects. We manipulated the conceptual distance between the learning stimuli in the learning phase and between the learning and generalization phase stimuli for object and relational nouns. Results showed that both populations had rather similar performance profile when matched on their cognitive skills (low- vs. high-functioning). Unexpectedly, ID children's performance was equivalent for relations and better for objects compared to their TD peers' performance. However, when controlling for chronological age, the difference between ID and TD children disappeared in the case of object categories and was better understood by TD children in the case of relations. We discuss the role of conceptual distance on participants' conceptual generalization as a function of their intellectual abilities and cognitive functioning.

[1]  D. Gentner,et al.  Comparison in the Development of Categories , 1999 .

[2]  O. Hetzroni,et al.  Learning new relational categories by children with autism spectrum disorders, children with typical development and children with intellectual disabilities: effects of comparison and familiarity on systematicity. , 2019, Journal of intellectual disability research : JIDR.

[3]  P. Foreman,et al.  Using augmentative communication with infants and young children with Down syndrome. , 1998, Down's syndrome, research and practice : the journal of the Sarah Duffen Centre.

[4]  Dedre Gentner,et al.  Comparison Facilitates Children's Learning of Names for Parts , 2007 .

[5]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  The importance of shape in early lexical learning , 1988 .

[6]  Florencia K. Anggoro,et al.  Structure mapping and relational language support children's learning of relational categories. , 2011, Child development.

[7]  D. G. Kemler Classification in young and retarded children: the primacy of overall similarity relations. , 1982, Child development.

[8]  C. Mervis,et al.  Effects of receptive and expressive training of category labels on generalized learning by severely mentally retarded children. , 1986, American journal of mental deficiency.

[9]  B. Orobio de Castro,et al.  Executive functions and processing speed in children with mild to borderline intellectual disabilities and externalizing behavior problems , 2017, Child neuropsychology : a journal on normal and abnormal development in childhood and adolescence.

[10]  J. Johnston,et al.  Relation between mental age and vocabulary development among children with mild mental retardation. , 1993, American journal of mental retardation : AJMR.

[11]  Robin S. Chapman,et al.  Language development in children and adolescents with Down syndrome , 1997 .

[12]  M. Budoff,et al.  Differences between EMR and nonretarded children in fluency and quality of verbal associations. , 1975, American journal of mental deficiency.

[13]  Sandra R. Waxman,et al.  Assumptions about Word Meaning: Individuation and Basic‐Level Kinds , 1993 .

[14]  G. Murphy,et al.  The Big Book of Concepts , 2002 .

[15]  Dean Sutherland,et al.  Three children with autism spectrum disorder learn to perform a three-step communication sequence using an iPad®-based speech-generating device , 2014, International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience.

[16]  D. Gentner,et al.  More evidence for a relational shift in the development of analogy: Children's performance on a causal-mapping task , 1998 .

[17]  Arnaud Witt,et al.  Generalization of novel names for relations in comparison settings: the role of conceptual distance during learning and at test , 2018, CogSci.

[18]  Eleanor Rosch,et al.  Principles of Categorization , 1978 .

[19]  Micah B. Goldwater,et al.  Relational categories as a bridge between cognitive and educational research. , 2016, Psychological bulletin.

[20]  R S Chapman,et al.  Language skills of children and adolescents with Down syndrome: I. Comprehension. , 1991, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[21]  Sandra R Waxman,et al.  The role of comparison in the extension of novel adjectives. , 2000, Developmental psychology.

[22]  J. Thibaut,et al.  Young children's learning of relational categories: multiple comparisons and their cognitive constraints , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[23]  D. Gentner,et al.  Making a silk purse out of two sow's ears: young children's use of comparison in category learning. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[24]  Susan C. Hupp Use of multiple exemplars in object concept training: How many are sufficient?* , 1986 .

[25]  J. Thibaut,et al.  The benefits and costs of comparisons in a novel object categorization task: Interactions with development , 2013, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[26]  Elizabeth E. Biggs,et al.  Systematic Review of Interventions Involving Aided AAC Modeling for Children With Complex Communication Needs. , 2018, American journal on intellectual and developmental disabilities.

[27]  S. Lanfranchi,et al.  Executive function in adolescents with Down Syndrome. , 2010, Journal of intellectual disability research : JIDR.

[28]  K. Holyoak,et al.  Children's development of analogical reasoning: insights from scene analogy problems. , 2006, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[29]  Dedre Gentner,et al.  Bootstrapping the Mind: Analogical Processes and Symbol Systems , 2010, Cogn. Sci..

[30]  Timothy J. Nokes-Malach,et al.  Learning Through Case Comparisons: A Meta-Analytic Review , 2013 .

[31]  Brett K. Hayes,et al.  Concept acquisition in children with mild intellectual disability: Factors affecting the abstraction of prototypical information , 2000 .

[32]  Wayne D. Gray,et al.  Basic objects in natural categories , 1976, Cognitive Psychology.

[33]  E. Kontu,et al.  Teaching Children with Intellectual Disabilities: Analysis of Research-Based Recommendations. , 2016 .

[34]  J. Pozo,et al.  On the Road to Graphicacy: The learning of graphical representation systems , 2004 .

[35]  H. Tager-Flusberg Constraints on the Representation of Word Meaning: Evidence From Autistic and Mentally Retarded Children , 1986 .

[36]  Jae H. Paik,et al.  Korean- and English-speaking children use cross-situational information to learn novel predicate terms* , 2008, Journal of Child Language.

[37]  C. Mervis,et al.  Acquisition of basic object categories by severely handicapped children. , 1982, Child development.

[38]  E. Rosch Cognitive Representations of Semantic Categories. , 1975 .

[39]  Arnaud Witt,et al.  Generalizing novel names in comparison settings: role of conceptual distance during learning and at test , 2017, CogSci.

[40]  H. Tager-Flusberg The conceptual basis for referential word meaning in children with autism. , 1985, Child development.

[41]  A. Salvi,et al.  Joint attention behaviours and vocabulary development in children with Down syndrome. , 2015, Journal of intellectual disability research : JIDR.