Eye movements in forced-choice recognition: Absolute judgments can preclude relative judgments

Abstract Forced choice recognition is usually assumed to involve a relative judgment process in which each test alternative is matched to memory and the one with the highest memory strength is selected. We monitored eye movements during a forced-choice recognition test to determine if absolute judgments also play a role; that is, do participants ever select an item because its memory strength exceeds an absolute criterion without comparing it to the other item? The results strongly supported a role for absolute judgments. Participants sometimes selected a response without looking at one of the test alternatives, and they were most likely to do this when they looked at the target first and correctly selected it as the studied item. Participants were also more accurate when they looked at the target first than when they looked at the lure first, which would be expected if they sometimes failed to consider the actual target word because they made an incorrect absolute judgment that the lure was studied. Finally, response times were faster when the word selected as the studied item was considered first than when it was considered second; that is, correct responses were faster when the target was viewed first and errors were faster when the lure was viewed first. This would be expected if participants sometimes make absolute judgments that the first item was studied, thus eliminating the additional time needed to consider the second item.

[1]  Angela M. Pazzaglia,et al.  A critical comparison of discrete-state and continuous models of recognition memory: implications for recognition and beyond. , 2013, Psychological bulletin.

[2]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  Evidence for discrete-state processing in recognition memory , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[3]  A. Yonelinas,et al.  Separating sensitivity from response bias: implications of comparisons of yes-no and forced-choice tests for models and measures of recognition memory. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[4]  Larry R. Squire,et al.  Yes/No Recognition, Forced-choice Recognition, and the Human Hippocampus , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[5]  N. J. Gandhi,et al.  Interactions of eye and eyelid movements , 2011 .

[6]  M. Glanzer,et al.  Analysis of the word-frequency effect in recognition memory , 1976 .

[7]  Colleen M. Parks,et al.  Receiver operating characteristics (ROCs) in recognition memory: a review. , 2007, Psychological bulletin.

[8]  A. Criss The consequences of differentiation in episodic memory: Similarity and the strength based mirror effect , 2006 .

[9]  J. Wixted,et al.  Testing signal-detection models of yes/no and two-alternative forced-choice recognition memory. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[10]  E. Heit,et al.  Associative Recognition: a Case of Recall-to-reject Processing , 2022 .

[11]  P. J. Fay The effect of cigarette smoking on simple and choice reaction time to colored lights , 1936 .

[12]  T. Wickens Elementary Signal Detection Theory , 2001 .

[13]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  The Diffusion Decision Model: Theory and Data for Two-Choice Decision Tasks , 2008, Neural Computation.

[14]  Shane T. Mueller,et al.  Decision noise: An explanation for observed violations of signal detection theory , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[15]  Max Coltheart,et al.  The MRC Psycholinguistic Database , 1981 .

[16]  D. M. Green,et al.  On the equivalence of two recognition measures of short-term memory. , 1966, Psychological bulletin.

[17]  John T Wixted,et al.  Strong memories are hard to scale. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[18]  A. Bröder,et al.  Recognition ROCs are curvilinear-or are they? On premature arguments against the two-high-threshold model of recognition. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[19]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Time course of item and associative information: implications for global memory models. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[20]  L L Jacoby,et al.  Isolating the contributions of familiarity and source information to item recognition: a time course analysis. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[21]  David Kellen,et al.  Evaluating models of recognition memory using first- and second-choice responses , 2011 .

[22]  R. Harald Baayen,et al.  Analyzing linguistic data: a practical introduction to statistics using R, 1st Edition , 2008 .

[23]  R. Marsh,et al.  A decrement-to-familiarity interpretation of the revelation effect from forced-choice tests of recognition memory. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[24]  Matthew Duncan,et al.  Testing theories of recognition memory by predicting performance across paradigms. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[25]  Henrik Singmann,et al.  Further evidence for discrete-state mediation in recognition memory. , 2015, Experimental psychology.

[26]  D. Callan,et al.  Forced-choice associative recognition: implications for global-memory models , 1993 .

[27]  Angela M. Pazzaglia,et al.  Unequal-strength source zROC slopes reflect criteria placement and not (necessarily) memory processes. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[28]  W. Hockley Analysis of response time distributions in the study of cognitive processes. , 1984 .

[29]  J. Wixted Dual-process theory and signal-detection theory of recognition memory. , 2007, Psychological review.

[30]  M Glanzer,et al.  The mirror effect in recognition memory , 1984, Memory & cognition.

[31]  J. Swets,et al.  A decision-making theory of visual detection. , 1954, Psychological review.

[32]  R. O’Reilly,et al.  Modeling hippocampal and neocortical contributions to recognition memory: a complementary-learning-systems approach. , 2003, Psychological review.

[33]  Robert T Knight,et al.  The contribution of recollection and familiarity to yes–no and forced-choice recognition tests in healthy subjects and amnesics , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  Colleen M. Parks,et al.  Evidence for a memory threshold in second-choice recognition memory responses , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[35]  Ian Krajbich,et al.  Visual fixations and the computation and comparison of value in simple choice , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[36]  D. Bates,et al.  Linear Mixed-Effects Models using 'Eigen' and S4 , 2015 .

[37]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  A direct test of the differentiation mechanism: REM, BCDMEM, and the strength-based mirror effect in recognition memory. , 2010, Journal of memory and language.

[38]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Modeling confidence and response time in recognition memory. , 2009, Psychological review.

[39]  T. Zandt ROC curves and confidence judgments in recognition memory. , 2000 .