Male satin bowerbirds, Ptilonorhynchus violaceus, adjust their display intensity in response to female startling: an experiment with robotic females
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] L. Dill,et al. Male mating strategies under predation risk: do females call the shots? , 1999 .
[2] J. Evans,et al. Female behaviour mediates male courtship under predation risk in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata) , 2002, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[3] J. Travis,et al. Social context and courtship flexibility in male sailfin mollies, Poecilia latipinna (Pisces: Poecillidae) , 1989, Animal Behaviour.
[4] Barney Luttbeg,et al. A Comparative Bayes tactic for mate assessment and choice , 1996 .
[5] T. Balsby,et al. Female behaviour affects male courtship in whitethroats, Sylvia communis: an interactive experiment using visual and acoustic cues , 2002, Animal Behaviour.
[6] H. Keselman,et al. Multiple Comparison Procedures , 2005 .
[7] A. Göth,et al. Social responses without early experience: Australian brush-turkey chicks use specific visual cues to aggregate with conspecifics , 2004, Journal of Experimental Biology.
[8] G. Borgia,et al. Coevolution of elaborated male display traits in the spotted bowerbird: an experimental test of the threat reduction hypothesis , 1998, Animal Behaviour.
[9] U. Candolin. Predation risk affects courtship and attractiveness of competing threespine stickleback males , 1997, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[10] R. Meldola. Sexual Selection , 1871, Nature.
[11] L. Meffert,et al. A test of speciation via sexual selection on female preferences , 2002, Animal Behaviour.
[12] G. Borgia,et al. Co–option of male courtship signals from aggressive display in bowerbirds , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[13] R. Richardson. Shock sensitization of startle: learned or unlearned fear? , 2000, Behavioural Brain Research.
[14] A. Cowling,et al. Do temperature and social environment interact to affect call rate in frogs ( Crinia signifera ) ? , 2004 .
[15] J. Endler. Predation, light intensity and courtship behaviour in Poecilia reticulata (Pisces: Poeciliidae) , 1987, Animal Behaviour.
[16] G. Borgia. SEXUAL SELECTION AND THE EVOLUTION OF MATING SYSTEMS , 1979 .
[17] R. Lande. Models of speciation by sexual selection on polygenic traits. , 1981, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[18] A. King,et al. Epigenesis of cowbird song—A joint endeavour of males and females , 1983, Nature.
[19] A. King,et al. Female visual displays affect the development of male song in the cowbird , 1988, Nature.
[20] R. Littell. SAS System for Mixed Models , 1996 .
[21] H. Gerhardt,et al. Female mate choice in treefrogs: static and dynamic acoustic criteria , 1991, Animal Behaviour.
[22] G. Borgia,et al. Sexual Selection, Mating Systems, and the Evolution of Avian Acoustical Displays , 1986, The American Naturalist.
[23] P. Lang. The emotion probe. Studies of motivation and attention. , 1995, The American psychologist.
[24] M. Sullivan. Assessing female choice for mates when the males' characters vary during the sampling period , 1990, Animal Behaviour.
[25] G Borgia. The Cost of Display in the Non-Resource-Based Mating System of the Satin Bowerbird , 1993, The American Naturalist.
[26] B. Webb. What does robotics offer animal behaviour? , 2000, Animal Behaviour.
[27] Gail L. Patricelli,et al. Female signals enhance the efficiency of mate assessment in satin bowerbirds (Ptilonorhynchus violaceus) , 2004 .
[28] U. Mueller,et al. Bower Destruction, Decoration Stealing and Female Choice in the Spotted Bowerbird Chlamydera maculata , 1992 .
[29] Ross B. Cunningham,et al. Do temperature and social environment interact to affect call rate in frogs (Crinia signifera) , 2004 .
[30] G. Patricelli,et al. Sexual selection: Male displays adjusted to female's response , 2002, Nature.
[31] J. Carranza,et al. Effects of male dominance and courtship display on female choice in the ring-necked pheasant , 1999, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[32] A. Lotem,et al. The overlooked signaling component of nonsignaling behavior , 1999 .
[33] G. Patricelli,et al. Multiple male traits interact: attractive bower decorations facilitate attractive behavioural displays in satin bowerbirds , 2003, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[34] Jack W. Bradbury,et al. Principles of Animal Communication , 1998 .
[35] G. Patricelli,et al. Complex Mate Searching in the Satin Bowerbird Ptilonorhynchus violaceus , 2001, The American Naturalist.
[36] E. P.. Animal Behaviour , 1901, Nature.
[37] Manfred Milinski,et al. Sexual selection and the evolution of female choice , 1994 .
[38] A. Pilastro,et al. Armaments and ornaments: an evolutionary explanation of traits of dual utility , 1996 .
[39] D. Blumstein. Acoustic Communication in Insects and Anurans : Common Problems and Diverse Solutions , 2002 .
[40] P. Weatherhead,et al. Seeing red: uncovering coverable badges in red-winged blackbirds , 1992, Animal Behaviour.
[41] K. D. Long,et al. Changes in male guppy courting distance in response to a fluctuating light environment , 1998, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[42] G. Borgia. Why Do Bowerbirds Build Bowers , 1995 .
[43] T. E. Moore,et al. Sexual Selection and Reproductive Competition in Insects , 1979 .
[44] W. Kirchner,et al. How honeybees perceive communication dances, studied by means of a mechanical model , 1992, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[45] Younglim Lee,et al. Amygdala and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis: differential roles in fear and anxiety measured with the acoustic startle reflex. , 1997, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[46] M. Koch,et al. The neurobiology of startle , 1999, Progress in Neurobiology.
[47] J. Godin. Predation risk and alternative mating tactics in male Trinidadian guppies (Poecilia reticulata) , 1995, Oecologia.
[48] G. Borgia. Threat Reduction as a Cause of Differences in Bower Architecture, Bower Decoration and Male Display in Two Closely-Related Bowerbirds Chlamydera nuchalis and C. maculata , 1995 .
[49] Y. Iwasa,et al. THE EVOLUTION OF COSTLY MATE PREFERENCES II. THE “HANDICAP” PRINCIPLE , 1991, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.
[50] A. Tamhane,et al. Multiple Comparison Procedures , 1989 .
[51] N. Santangelo. Courtship in the monogamous convict cichlid; what are individuals saying to rejected and selected mates? , 2005, Animal Behaviour.
[52] G. Borgia,et al. Male courtship vocalizations as cues for mate choice in the Satin Bowerbird (Ptilonorhynchus violaceus) , 1986 .
[53] T. Guilford,et al. Design of an intention signal in the bluehead wrasse (Thalassoma bifasciatum) , 1994, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[54] A. Grafen. Biological signals as handicaps. , 1990, Journal of theoretical biology.
[55] G. Patricelli,et al. Variable female preferences drive complex male displays , 2004, Nature.
[56] G. Patricelli,et al. Dynamic mate-searching tactic allows female satin bowerbirds Ptilonorhynchus violaceus to reduce searching , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[57] P. Narins,et al. Bimodal signal requisite for agonistic behavior in a dart-poison frog, Epipedobates femoralis , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[58] M. Geyer,et al. Aging Effects on the Startle Response and Startle Plasticity in Fischer F344 Rats , 1998, Neurobiology of Aging.