Computational nature of human adaptive control during learning of reaching movements in force fields

Abstract. Learning to make reaching movements in force fields was used as a paradigm to explore the system architecture of the biological adaptive controller. We compared the performance of a number of candidate control systems that acted on a model of the neuromuscular system of the human arm and asked how well the dynamics of the candidate system compared with the movement characteristics of 16 subjects. We found that control via a supra-spinal system that utilized an adaptive inverse model resulted in dynamics that were similar to that observed in our subjects, but lacked essential characteristics. These characteristics pointed to a different architecture where descending commands were influenced by an adaptive forward model. However, we found that control via a forward model alone also resulted in dynamics that did not match the behavior of the human arm. We considered a third control architecture where a forward model was used in conjunction with an inverse model and found that the resulting dynamics were remarkably similar to that observed in the experimental data. The essential property of this control architecture was that it predicted a complex pattern of near-discontinuities in hand trajectory in the novel force field. A nearly identical pattern was observed in our subjects, suggesting that generation of descending motor commands was likely through a control system architecture that included both adaptive forward and inverse models. We found that as subjects learned to make reaching movements, adaptation rates for the forward and inverse models could be independently estimated and the resulting changes in performance of subjects from movement to movement could be accurately accounted for. Results suggested that the adaptation of the forward model played a dominant role in the motor learning of subjects. After a period of consolidation, the rates of adaptation in the internal models were significantly larger than those observed before the memory had consolidated. This suggested that consolidation of motor memory coincided with freeing of certain computational resources for subsequent learning.

[1]  R. Sperry Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. , 1950, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[2]  R. G. Lee,et al.  Motor Responses to Sudden Limb Displacements in Primates with Specific CNS Lesions and in Human Patients with Motor System Disorders , 1975, Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences / Journal Canadien des Sciences Neurologiques.

[3]  T. Flash,et al.  The coordination of arm movements: an experimentally confirmed mathematical model , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[4]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Neural, mechanical, and geometric factors subserving arm posture in humans , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[5]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Adaptation and learning in control of voluntary movement by the central nervous system , 1988, Adv. Robotics.

[6]  F. Zajac Muscle and tendon: properties, models, scaling, and application to biomechanics and motor control. , 1989, Critical reviews in biomedical engineering.

[7]  C. Atkeson,et al.  Learning arm kinematics and dynamics. , 1989, Annual review of neuroscience.

[8]  Jean-Jacques E. Slotine,et al.  Stable Adaptive Teleoperation , 1990, 1990 American Control Conference.

[9]  Reza Shadmehr Learning Virtual Equilibrium Trajectories for Control of a Robot Arm , 1990, Neural Computation.

[10]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Forward Models: Supervised Learning with a Distal Teacher , 1992, Cogn. Sci..

[11]  M. Kawato,et al.  The cerebellum and VOR/OKR learning models , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[12]  M. Kawato,et al.  Inverse-dynamics model eye movement control by Purkinje cells in the cerebellum , 1993, Nature.

[13]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Is the cerebellum a smith predictor? , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[14]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  A neural network model for arm trajectory formation using forward and inverse dynamics models , 1993, Neural Networks.

[15]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[16]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  A Model of the Learning of Arm Trajectories from Spatial Deviations , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[17]  P Cordo,et al.  Proprioceptive coordination of movement sequences: role of velocity and position information. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  D. Bodznick,et al.  An adaptive filter that cancels self-induced noise in the electrosensory and lateral line mechanosensory systems of fish , 1994, Neuroscience Letters.

[19]  Peter Ford Dominey,et al.  Motor imagery of a lateralized sequential task is asymmetrically slowed in hemi-Parkinson's patients , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  S. Delp,et al.  Variation of muscle moment arms with elbow and forearm position. , 1995, Journal of biomechanics.

[21]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  An internal model for sensorimotor integration. , 1995, Science.

[22]  S P Wise,et al.  Distributed modular architectures linking basal ganglia, cerebellum, and cerebral cortex: their role in planning and controlling action. , 1995, Cerebral cortex.

[23]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Sense of muscular effort and somesthetic afferent information in humans. , 1995, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[24]  J. Slotine,et al.  On metric observers for nonlinear systems , 1996, Proceeding of the 1996 IEEE International Conference on Control Applications IEEE International Conference on Control Applications held together with IEEE International Symposium on Intelligent Contro.

[25]  A. Sirigu,et al.  The Mental Representation of Hand Movements After Parietal Cortex Damage , 1996, Science.

[26]  Jean-Jacques E Slotine,et al.  The intermediate cerebellum may function as a wave-variable processor , 1996, Neuroscience Letters.

[27]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Forward Models for Physiological Motor Control , 1996, Neural Networks.

[28]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Equilibrium-Point Control Hypothesis Examined by Measured Arm Stiffness During Multijoint Movement , 1996, Science.

[29]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Consolidation in human motor memory , 1996, Nature.

[30]  Christian Darlot,et al.  Computation of inverse dynamics for the control of movements , 1996, Biological Cybernetics.

[31]  V. Han,et al.  Synaptic plasticity in a cerebellum-like structure depends on temporal order , 1997, Nature.

[32]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Neural correlates of motor memory consolidation. , 1997, Science.

[33]  T. Ebner,et al.  What features of visually guided arm movements are encoded in the simple spike discharge of cerebellar Purkinje cells? , 1997, Progress in brain research.

[34]  J. Fuster The Prefrontal Cortex , 1997 .

[35]  Sybert H. Stroeve,et al.  A learning feedback and feedforward neuromuscular control model for two degrees of freedom human arm movements , 1997 .

[36]  Jacques Droulez,et al.  Does the brain use sliding variables for the control of movements? , 1997, Biological Cybernetics.

[37]  J R Flanagan,et al.  The Role of Internal Models in Motion Planning and Control: Evidence from Grip Force Adjustments during Movements of Hand-Held Loads , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  T. Brashers-Krug,et al.  Functional Stages in the Formation of Human Long-Term Motor Memory , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  Gideon F. Inbar,et al.  A model for learning human reaching movements , 1996, Biological Cybernetics.

[40]  D M Wolpert,et al.  Predicting the Consequences of Our Own Actions: The Role of Sensorimotor Context Estimation , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  J. Bloedel,et al.  Effects of cerebellar nuclear inactivation on the learning of a complex forelimb movement in cats. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  G. Stelmach,et al.  Ataxia reflected in the simulated movements of patients with cerebellar lesions , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[43]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Internal models in the cerebellum , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[44]  W. T. Thach,et al.  Simple spike activity predicts occurrence of complex spikes in cerebellar Purkinje cells , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[45]  M. Arbib,et al.  Role of the cerebellum in reaching movements in humans. I. Distributed inverse dynamics control , 1998, The European journal of neuroscience.

[46]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Maintaining internal representations: the role of the human superior parietal lobe , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[47]  D J Ostry,et al.  Are complex control signals required for human arm movement? , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  M. Kawato,et al.  Temporal firing patterns of Purkinje cells in the cerebellar ventral paraflocculus during ocular following responses in monkeys I. Simple spikes. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[49]  D M Wolpert,et al.  Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control , 1998, Neural Networks.

[50]  James C. Houk,et al.  A Cerebellar Model of Timing and Prediction in the Control of Reaching , 1999, Neural Computation.