Pupil response hazard rates predict perceived gaze durations
暂无分享,去创建一个
Alan Johnston | Isabelle Mareschal | Nicola Binetti | Charlotte Harrison | A. Johnston | I. Mareschal | N. Binetti | Charlotte Harrison
[1] J. Haxby,et al. Distinct representations of eye gaze and identity in the distributed human neural system for face perception , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[2] F. Vidal,et al. Event-Related Potentials as Indices of Time Processing: A Review , 2004 .
[3] Franck Vidal,et al. The supplementary motor area in motor and sensory timing: evidence from slow brain potential changes , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.
[4] Kwun Kei Ng,et al. Temporal Accumulation and Decision Processes in the Duration Bisection Task Revealed by Contingent Negative Variation , 2011, Front. Integr. Neurosci..
[5] S. Grondin. Timing and time perception: A review of recent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoretical directions , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[6] Robert C. Wolpert,et al. A Review of the , 1985 .
[7] S. Sirois,et al. Pupillometry , 2012, Eye Movement Research.
[8] Paul D. Kieffaber,et al. The neural correlates of temporal judgments in the duration bisection task , 2013, Neuropsychologia.
[9] A. J. Mistlin,et al. Visual cells in the temporal cortex sensitive to face view and gaze direction , 1985, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.
[10] Antoine Coutrot,et al. Pupil dilation as an index of preferred mutual gaze duration , 2016, Royal Society Open Science.
[11] D. Eagleman,et al. The Effect of Predictability on Subjective Duration , 2007, PloS one.
[12] D. Eagleman. Human time perception and its illusions , 2008, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[13] E. Stauffer,et al. Post‐transcriptional regulation in root development , 2014, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. RNA.
[14] M. Shadlen,et al. A representation of the hazard rate of elapsed time in macaque area LIP , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[15] Mark H. Johnson,et al. The eye contact effect: mechanisms and development , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[16] D. Perrett,et al. Being the target of another’s emotion: a PET study , 2003, Neuropsychologia.
[17] R. Dolan,et al. Separate Coding of Different Gaze Directions in the Superior Temporal Sulcus and Inferior Parietal Lobule , 2007, Current Biology.
[18] R. Church,et al. Representation of time , 1990, Cognition.
[19] M. Shadlen,et al. Representation of Time by Neurons in the Posterior Parietal Cortex of the Macaque , 2003, Neuron.
[20] C. Halldin,et al. A PET study of , 2000, The international journal of neuropsychopharmacology.
[21] J. Nachmias. The role of virtual standards in visual discrimination , 2006, Vision Research.
[22] D. Perrett,et al. Understanding the intentions of others from visual signals: Neurophysiological evidence. , 1994 .
[23] Barbara G. Shinn-Cunningham,et al. The pupil response is sensitive to divided attention during speech processing , 2014, Hearing Research.
[24] M. Paulus,et al. Decision making, impulsivity and time perception , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[25] A. Young,et al. Reading the mind from eye gaze , 2002, Neuropsychologia.
[26] G. McCarthy,et al. Neural basis of eye gaze processing deficits in autism. , 2005, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[27] Dean V Buonomano,et al. Influence of the interstimulus interval on temporal processing and learning: testing the state-dependent network model , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[28] A. Nobre,et al. The hazards of time , 2007, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[29] L. Maffei,et al. I. Neurophysiological evidence , 1982 .
[30] C. Koch,et al. Pupil dilation reflects perceptual selection and predicts subsequent stability in perceptual rivalry , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[31] M. Treisman. Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval. Implications for a model of the "internal clock". , 1963, Psychological monographs.