Pupil response hazard rates predict perceived gaze durations

We investigated the mechanisms for evaluating perceived gaze-shift duration. Timing relies on the accumulation of endogenous physiological signals. Here we focused on arousal, measured through pupil dilation, as a candidate timing signal. Participants timed gaze-shifts performed by face stimuli in a Standard/Probe comparison task. Pupil responses were binned according to “Longer/Shorter” judgements in trials where Standard and Probe were identical. This ensured that pupil responses reflected endogenous arousal fluctuations opposed to differences in stimulus content. We found that pupil hazard rates predicted the classification of sub-second intervals (steeper dilation = “Longer” classifications). This shows that the accumulation of endogenous arousal signals informs gaze-shift timing judgements. We also found that participants relied exclusively on the 2nd stimulus to perform the classification, providing insights into timing strategies under conditions of maximum uncertainty. We observed no dissociation in pupil responses when timing equivalent neutral spatial displacements, indicating that a stimulus-dependent timer exploits arousal to time gaze-shifts.

[1]  J. Haxby,et al.  Distinct representations of eye gaze and identity in the distributed human neural system for face perception , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[2]  F. Vidal,et al.  Event-Related Potentials as Indices of Time Processing: A Review , 2004 .

[3]  Franck Vidal,et al.  The supplementary motor area in motor and sensory timing: evidence from slow brain potential changes , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  Kwun Kei Ng,et al.  Temporal Accumulation and Decision Processes in the Duration Bisection Task Revealed by Contingent Negative Variation , 2011, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[5]  S. Grondin Timing and time perception: A review of recent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoretical directions , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[6]  Robert C. Wolpert,et al.  A Review of the , 1985 .

[7]  S. Sirois,et al.  Pupillometry , 2012, Eye Movement Research.

[8]  Paul D. Kieffaber,et al.  The neural correlates of temporal judgments in the duration bisection task , 2013, Neuropsychologia.

[9]  A. J. Mistlin,et al.  Visual cells in the temporal cortex sensitive to face view and gaze direction , 1985, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[10]  Antoine Coutrot,et al.  Pupil dilation as an index of preferred mutual gaze duration , 2016, Royal Society Open Science.

[11]  D. Eagleman,et al.  The Effect of Predictability on Subjective Duration , 2007, PloS one.

[12]  D. Eagleman Human time perception and its illusions , 2008, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[13]  E. Stauffer,et al.  Post‐transcriptional regulation in root development , 2014, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. RNA.

[14]  M. Shadlen,et al.  A representation of the hazard rate of elapsed time in macaque area LIP , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[15]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  The eye contact effect: mechanisms and development , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[16]  D. Perrett,et al.  Being the target of another’s emotion: a PET study , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  R. Dolan,et al.  Separate Coding of Different Gaze Directions in the Superior Temporal Sulcus and Inferior Parietal Lobule , 2007, Current Biology.

[18]  R. Church,et al.  Representation of time , 1990, Cognition.

[19]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Representation of Time by Neurons in the Posterior Parietal Cortex of the Macaque , 2003, Neuron.

[20]  C. Halldin,et al.  A PET study of , 2000, The international journal of neuropsychopharmacology.

[21]  J. Nachmias The role of virtual standards in visual discrimination , 2006, Vision Research.

[22]  D. Perrett,et al.  Understanding the intentions of others from visual signals: Neurophysiological evidence. , 1994 .

[23]  Barbara G. Shinn-Cunningham,et al.  The pupil response is sensitive to divided attention during speech processing , 2014, Hearing Research.

[24]  M. Paulus,et al.  Decision making, impulsivity and time perception , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[25]  A. Young,et al.  Reading the mind from eye gaze , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[26]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Neural basis of eye gaze processing deficits in autism. , 2005, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[27]  Dean V Buonomano,et al.  Influence of the interstimulus interval on temporal processing and learning: testing the state-dependent network model , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[28]  A. Nobre,et al.  The hazards of time , 2007, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[29]  L. Maffei,et al.  I. Neurophysiological evidence , 1982 .

[30]  C. Koch,et al.  Pupil dilation reflects perceptual selection and predicts subsequent stability in perceptual rivalry , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[31]  M. Treisman Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval. Implications for a model of the "internal clock". , 1963, Psychological monographs.