Distinct short-term and long-term adaptation to reduce saccade size in monkey.

In monkeys, saccades that repeatedly overshoot their targets adapt to become smaller by the time the monkey has made 1,000-2,000 saccades. In life, adaptation must keep movements accurate for long periods of time. Previous work describes only saccade adaptation that occurs within a few hours. Here we describe long-term saccade adaptation elicited in three monkeys by 19 days of training. Each day a monkey made saccades to track 16 degrees leftward and rightward target movements. During saccades, the target stepped back toward its starting position 6.4 degrees (40%) in two monkeys or 8 degrees (50%) in the third. After each day's adaptation, we blindfolded the monkey with goggles and returned it to its cage overnight. We found that adapting saccades for 19 days elicited significantly larger, long-lasting reduction in saccade size than did adapting for only 1 day. Further, after 19 days of adaptation we could elicit additional, apparently normal, short-term reduction in saccade size by increasing the size of the intra-saccade target movement. In contrast, we could elicit only small additional size reduction after only 1 day of adaptation. A simple model using separate short- and long-term adaptation mechanisms can reproduce many of the features of saccade gain exhibited by monkeys during a 19-day adaptation. We conclude that there is a long-term saccade-adaptation mechanism that is distinct from the well-characterized short-term system and that this newly recognized system is responsible for long-term maintenance of saccade accuracy.

[1]  L. Dell’Osso,et al.  Saccadic system plasticity in humans , 1978, Annals of neurology.

[2]  P. Thier,et al.  Saccadic Dysmetria and Adaptation after Lesions of the Cerebellar Cortex , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  Kaoru Yoshida,et al.  Changes in cerebellar fastigial burst activity related to saccadic gain adaptation in the monkey , 2003, Neuroscience Research.

[4]  H Deubel,et al.  Adaptive gain control of saccadic eye movements. , 1986, Human neurobiology.

[5]  S. Lisberger,et al.  The Cerebellum: A Neuronal Learning Machine? , 1996, Science.

[6]  A. Opstal,et al.  Monkey Superior Colliculus Activity During Short-Term Saccadic Adaptation , 1997, Brain Research Bulletin.

[7]  T Fujikado,et al.  Topography of the oculomotor area of the cerebellar vermis in macaques as determined by microstimulation. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Cerebellar Influences on Saccade Plasticity , 2002, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[9]  L. Optican,et al.  Cerebellar-dependent adaptive control of primate saccadic system. , 1980, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  W. B. Templeton,et al.  Saccadic plasticity: parametric adaptive control by retinal feedback. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  D. M. Broussard,et al.  The Site of a Motor Memory Shifts with Consolidation , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  Edward S Boyden,et al.  Active Reversal of Motor Memories Reveals Rules Governing Memory Encoding , 2003, Neuron.

[13]  A. Fuchs,et al.  A method for measuring horizontal and vertical eye movement chronically in the monkey. , 1966, Journal of applied physiology.

[14]  W. King,et al.  Rapid Motor Learning in the Translational Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  D. Zee,et al.  Effects of lesions of the oculomotor vermis on eye movements in primate: saccades. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  Kaoru Yoshida,et al.  Memory of Learning Facilitates Saccadic Adaptation in the Monkey , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  Scott E. Bevans,et al.  Effect of visual error size on saccade adaptation in monkey. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  H. Noda,et al.  Afferent and efferent connections of the oculomotor region of the fastigial nucleus in the macaque monkey , 1990, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[19]  E. D’Angelo,et al.  Beyond parallel fiber LTD: the diversity of synaptic and non-synaptic plasticity in the cerebellum , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[20]  C. Scudder,et al.  Adaptive modification of saccade size produces correlated changes in the discharges of fastigial nucleus neurons. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  T Anstis,et al.  Saccadic plasticity: parametric adaptive control by retinal feedback. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[22]  G. Kommerell,et al.  Adaptive programming of phasic and tonic components in saccadic eye movements. Investigations of patients with abducens palsy. , 1976, Investigative ophthalmology.

[23]  F. Robinson,et al.  Non-visual information does not drive saccade gain adaptation in monkeys , 2002, Brain Research.

[24]  Glenn E. Weisfeld,et al.  Parametric adjustment to a shifting target alternating with saccades to a stationary reference point , 1972 .

[25]  D. Robinson,et al.  A METHOD OF MEASURING EYE MOVEMENT USING A SCLERAL SEARCH COIL IN A MAGNETIC FIELD. , 1963, IEEE transactions on bio-medical engineering.

[26]  S. C. Mclaughlin Parametric adjustment in saccadic eye movements , 1967 .

[27]  H. Noda,et al.  Afferent and efferent connections of the oculomotor cerebellar vermis in the macaque monkey , 1987, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[28]  M. Horne,et al.  Ultrastructural change at rat cerebellothalamic synapses associated with volitional motor adaptation , 1999, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[29]  Y. Hirata,et al.  Memory retention of vestibuloocular reflex motor learning in squirrel monkeys , 2004, Neuroreport.

[30]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Characteristics of saccadic gain adaptation in rhesus macaques. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.