Hippocampal subfield volumes contribute to working memory interference control in aging: Evidence from longitudinal associations over 5 years

In memory, familiar but no longer relevant information may disrupt encoding and retrieval of to-be-learned information. While it has been demonstrated that the ability to resolve proactive interference (PI) in working memory (WM) is reduced in aging, the neuroanatomical components of this decline have yet to be determined. Hippocampal (HC) involvement in age- related decline in control of PI is currently not known. In particular, the association between HC subfield volumes and control of PI in WM has not been examined previously. Here we investigate the associations between mean level and 5-year trajectories of gray matter subfield volumes and PI in WM across the adult life span (N = 157). Longitudinal analyses over 5- years across all participants revealed that reduced volume in the subiculum was related to impaired control of PI. Age-stratified analyses showed that this association was most pronounced in older adults. Furthermore, we found that in older adults the effect of age on PI was mediated by GM volume in the HC. The current results show that HC volume is associated with the ability to control PI in WM, and that these associations are modulated by age.

[1]  J. Persson,et al.  Automatic and effortful control of interference in working memory can be distinguished by unique behavioral and functional brain representations , 2022, NeuroImage.

[2]  J. Persson,et al.  The association between control of interference and white-matter integrity: A cross-sectional and longitudinal investigation , 2022, Neurobiology of Aging.

[3]  M. Noulhiane,et al.  Hippocampal subfield volumes and memory discrimination in the developing brain , 2021, Hippocampus.

[4]  Brice A. Kuhl,et al.  Abrupt hippocampal remapping signals resolution of memory interference , 2021, Nature Communications.

[5]  Michelle I. Nash,et al.  Pattern separation beyond the hippocampus: A high‐resolution whole‐brain investigation of mnemonic discrimination in healthy adults , 2021, Hippocampus.

[6]  Katherine R. Sherrill,et al.  Memory Reactivation during Learning Simultaneously Promotes Dentate Gyrus/CA2,3 Pattern Differentiation and CA1 Memory Integration , 2020, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[7]  D. Antonenko,et al.  Dentate Gyrus Volume Mediates the Effect of Fornix Microstructure on Memory Formation in Older Adults , 2020, Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience.

[8]  J. Persson,et al.  Proactive interference in working memory is related to adult age and cognitive factors: cross-sectional and longitudinal evidence from the Betula study , 2020, Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition.

[9]  L. Nyberg,et al.  Frontal Contribution to Hippocampal Hyperactivity During Memory Encoding in Aging , 2019, Front. Mol. Neurosci..

[10]  Jonas Persson,et al.  Interference Control in Working Memory Is Associated with Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex Volume , 2019, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[11]  M. Rugg,et al.  Neural Dedifferentiation in the Aging Brain , 2019, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[12]  Nicola J. Ray,et al.  A Key Role for Subiculum-Fornix Connectivity in Recollection in Older Age , 2019, Front. Syst. Neurosci..

[13]  M. Gade,et al.  Inhibition in aging: What is preserved? What declines? A meta-analysis , 2017, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[14]  Keith A. Johnson,et al.  Dedifferentiation of caudate functional connectivity and striatal dopamine transporter density predict memory change in normal aging , 2018, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[15]  S. Lazar,et al.  Reduced interference in working memory following mindfulness training is associated with increases in hippocampal volume , 2018, Brain Imaging and Behavior.

[16]  L. Bäckman,et al.  Age-Differences in the Temporal Properties of Proactive Interference in Working Memory , 2017, Psychology and aging.

[17]  S-P Ku,et al.  Regional Specific Evidence for Memory-Load Dependent Activity in the Dorsal Subiculum and the Lateral Entorhinal Cortex , 2017, Front. Syst. Neurosci..

[18]  L. Nyberg,et al.  Longitudinal association between hippocampus atrophy and episodic-memory decline , 2017, Neurobiology of Aging.

[19]  Lindsey Lilienthal Individual differences in proactive interference in verbal and visuospatial working memory , 2017, Memory.

[20]  Teal S Eich,et al.  Dynamic Patterns of Brain Structure–Behavior Correlation Across the Lifespan , 2016, Cerebral cortex.

[21]  Koenraad Van Leemput,et al.  Bayesian longitudinal segmentation of hippocampal substructures in brain MRI using subject-specific atlases , 2016, NeuroImage.

[22]  Thomas Wolbers,et al.  The Human Dentate Gyrus Plays a Necessary Role in Discriminating New Memories , 2016, Current Biology.

[23]  Dharshan Kumaran,et al.  Strong Evidence for Pattern Separation in Human Dentate Gyrus , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  E. Rolls Pattern separation, completion, and categorisation in the hippocampus and neocortex , 2016, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[25]  C. Lustig,et al.  Questions of age differences in interference control: When and how, not if? , 2015, Brain Research.

[26]  David Badre,et al.  Ventral fronto-temporal pathway supporting cognitive control of episodic memory retrieval. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[27]  Andrew S. Whiteman,et al.  Hippocampal subfield and medial temporal cortical persistent activity during working memory reflects ongoing encoding , 2015, Front. Syst. Neurosci..

[28]  L. Howard,et al.  Changes in pattern completion – a key mechanism to explain age-related recognition memory deficits? , 2014, Cortex.

[29]  S. Czaja,et al.  An evaluation of deficits in semantic cueing and proactive and retroactive interference as early features of Alzheimer's disease. , 2014, The American journal of geriatric psychiatry : official journal of the American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry.

[30]  John Jonides,et al.  Frontal–Medial Temporal Interactions Mediate Transitions among Representational States in Short-Term Memory , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  Cornelius Weiller,et al.  Developmental change in proactive interference across the life span: evidence from two working memory tasks. , 2014, Developmental psychology.

[32]  Shauna M. Stark,et al.  Contributions of human hippocampal subfields to spatial and temporal pattern separation , 2014, Hippocampus.

[33]  Shauna M. Stark,et al.  A task to assess behavioral pattern separation (BPS) in humans: Data from healthy aging and mild cognitive impairment , 2013, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  Chantal E Stern,et al.  Contributions of the hippocampal subfields and entorhinal cortex to disambiguation during working memory , 2013, Hippocampus.

[35]  A. Hayes Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach , 2013 .

[36]  Jonas Persson,et al.  Imaging Fatigue of Interference Control Reveals the Neural Basis of Executive Resource Depletion , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[37]  Jonas Persson,et al.  Context-dependent switching between proactive and reactive working memory control mechanisms in the right inferior frontal gyrus , 2012, NeuroImage.

[38]  Jonas Persson,et al.  Longitudinal structure-function correlates in elderly reveal MTL dysfunction with cognitive decline. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[39]  Irene E. Nagel,et al.  Cortical thickness is linked to executive functioning in adulthood and aging , 2012, Human brain mapping.

[40]  M. Leszczyński How Does Hippocampus Contribute to Working Memory Processing? , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[41]  C. Stark,et al.  Pattern separation in the hippocampus , 2011, Trends in Neurosciences.

[42]  P. Verhaeghen Aging and Executive Control: Reports of a Demise Greatly Exaggerated , 2011, Current directions in psychological science.

[43]  Shauna M. Stark,et al.  Age-related memory deficits linked to circuit-specific disruptions in the hippocampus , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[44]  Frederick Verbruggen,et al.  Control of interference during working memory updating. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[45]  T. Braver,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Interference Control in Working Memory: Effects of Interference Expectancy and Fluid Intelligence , 2010, PloS one.

[46]  Lila Davachi,et al.  Are Representations in Working Memory Distinct From Representations in Long-Term Memory? , 2010, Psychological science.

[47]  A. Dale,et al.  One-Year Brain Atrophy Evident in Healthy Aging , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[48]  N. Cashdollar,et al.  Hippocampus-dependent and -independent theta-networks of active maintenance , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[49]  C Brock Kirwan,et al.  Visual object pattern separation deficits in nondemented older adults. , 2009, Learning & memory.

[50]  Olivier Potvin,et al.  Lesions of the dorsal subiculum and the dorsal hippocampus impaired pattern separation in a task using distinct and overlapping visual stimuli , 2009, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[51]  Jonas Persson,et al.  Mapping interference resolution across task domains: A shared control process in left inferior frontal gyrus , 2009, Brain Research.

[52]  P. Ebert,et al.  Proactive and retroactive interference in young adults, healthy older adults, and older adults with amnestic mild cognitive impairment , 2009, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[53]  Nikolai Axmacher,et al.  Interaction of working memory and long-term memory in the medial temporal lobe. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[54]  J. Myerson,et al.  Age differences in proactive interference, working memory, and abstract reasoning. , 2008, Psychology and aging.

[55]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Evidence for Segregated and Integrative Connectivity Patterns in the Human Basal Ganglia , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[56]  C. Stark,et al.  Pattern Separation in the Human Hippocampal CA3 and Dentate Gyrus , 2008, Science.

[57]  Marc G. Berman,et al.  Neural mechanisms of proactive interference-resolution , 2007, NeuroImage.

[58]  Jeremy B Caplan,et al.  Two distinct functional networks for successful resolution of proactive interference. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[59]  Charan Ranganath,et al.  Prefrontal Cortex and Long-Term Memory Encoding: An Integrative Review of Findings from Neuropsychology and Neuroimaging , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[60]  M. Moser,et al.  Pattern Separation in the Dentate Gyrus and CA3 of the Hippocampus , 2007, Science.

[61]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  Neurocognitive aging: prior memories hinder new hippocampal encoding , 2006, Trends in Neurosciences.

[62]  Neal J. Cohen,et al.  The Long and the Short of It: Relational Memory Impairments in Amnesia, Even at Short Lags , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[63]  John D E Gabrieli,et al.  Working memory and long‐term memory for faces: Evidence from fMRI and global amnesia for involvement of the medial temporal lobes , 2006, Hippocampus.

[64]  Lee Friedman,et al.  Report on a multicenter fMRI quality assurance protocol , 2006, Journal of magnetic resonance imaging : JMRI.

[65]  J. Jonides,et al.  Brain mechanisms of proactive interference in working memory , 2006, Neuroscience.

[66]  Anders M. Dale,et al.  Reliability in multi-site structural MRI studies: Effects of gradient non-linearity correction on phantom and human data , 2006, NeuroImage.

[67]  Michael F. Bunting,et al.  Proactive interference and item similarity in working memory. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[68]  David Badre,et al.  Frontal lobe mechanisms that resolve proactive interference. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[69]  C. Petten Relationship between hippocampal volume and memory ability in healthy individuals across the lifespan: review and meta-analysis , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[70]  James J. Knierim,et al.  Ensemble Dynamics of Hippocampal Regions CA3 and CA1 , 2004, Neuron.

[71]  Denise C. Park,et al.  Aging reduces neural specialization in ventral visual cortex. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[72]  S. Czaja,et al.  Cognitive rehabilitation of mildly impaired Alzheimer disease patients on cholinesterase inhibitors. , 2004, The American journal of geriatric psychiatry : official journal of the American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry.

[73]  L. Nyberg,et al.  Betula: A Prospective Cohort Study on Memory, Health and Aging , 2004 .

[74]  D. Head,et al.  Differential aging of the medial temporal lobe , 2004, Neurology.

[75]  D. Pandya,et al.  Efferent connections of the cingulate gyrus in the rhesus monkey , 1981, Experimental Brain Research.

[76]  C. Van Petten,et al.  Relationship between hippocampal volume and memory ability in healthy individuals across the lifespan: review and meta-analysis. , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[77]  Nikos Makris,et al.  Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[78]  Faith M. Gunning-Dixon,et al.  Differential aging of the human striatum: longitudinal evidence. , 2003, AJNR. American journal of neuroradiology.

[79]  C. Chabris,et al.  Neural mechanisms of general fluid intelligence , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[80]  R. J. Dolan,et al.  Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of Proactive Interference during Spoken Cued Recall , 2002, NeuroImage.

[81]  A. Dale,et al.  Whole Brain Segmentation Automated Labeling of Neuroanatomical Structures in the Human Brain , 2002, Neuron.

[82]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Medial Temporal Lobe Activity Associated with Active Maintenance of Novel Information , 2001, Neuron.

[83]  L Hasher,et al.  Working memory span and the role of proactive interference. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[84]  R. Engle,et al.  Working-memory capacity, proactive interference, and divided attention: limits on long-term memory retrieval. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[85]  Lynn Hasher,et al.  The role of interference in memory span , 1999, Memory & cognition.

[86]  D. Pandya,et al.  Fiber system linking the mid‐dorsolateral frontal cortex with the retrosplenial/presubicular region in the rhesus monkey , 1999, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[87]  Anders M. Dale,et al.  Cortical Surface-Based Analysis I. Segmentation and Surface Reconstruction , 1999, NeuroImage.

[88]  G. Winocur,et al.  Heightened Interference on Implicit, but Not Explicit, Tests of Negative Transfer: Evidence from Patients with Unilateral Temporal Lobe Lesions and Normal Old People , 1996, Brain and Cognition.

[89]  K. McDermott,et al.  Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. , 1995 .

[90]  D. Amaral,et al.  Perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices of the macaque monkey: Cortical afferents , 1994, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[91]  Lynn Hasher,et al.  Working Memory, Comprehension, and Aging: A Review and a New View , 1988 .

[92]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.