Input Elaboration, Head Faithfulness and Evidence for Representation in the Acquisition of Left-edge Clusters in West Germanic

Several recent investigations of the development of left-edge clusters in West Germanic languages have demonstrated that the relative sonority of adjacent consonants plays a key role in children’s reduction patterns (e.g. Fikkert 1994, Gilbers & Den Ouden 1994, Chin 1996, Barlow 1997, Bernhardt & Stemberger 1998, Gierut 1999, Ohala 1999, Gnanadesikan this volume). These authors have argued that, for a number of children, at the stage in development when only one member of a left-edge cluster is produced, it is the least sonorous segment that survives, regardless of where this segment appears in the target string or the structural position that it occupies (head, dependent, or appendix). To briefly illustrate, while the more sonorous /S/ 2 is lost in favour of the stop in /S/+stop clusters, /S/ is retained in /S/+sonorant clusters; similarly, the least sonorous stop survives in both /S/+stop and stop+sonorant clusters, in spite of the fact that it occurs in different positions in the two strings. To account for reduction patterns such as these, a structural difference between /S/-initial and stop-initial clusters need not be assumed. This would seem to fare well in view of much of the recent constraint-based literature which de-emphasises the role of prosodic constituency in favour of phonetically-based explanations of phonological phenomena (see e.g. Hamilton 1996, Wright 1996, Kochetov 1999, Steriade 1999, Cote 2000). In this paper, we focus on a second set of reduction patterns for left-edge clusters, one which is not addressed in most of the sonority-based literature on cluster reduction in child language (L1): these patterns reveal a preference for structural heads to survive. For example, while the stop is retained in clusters of the shape /S/+stop and stop+sonorant, it is the sonorant that survives in /S/+sonorant clusters. The only sources that we have found where explicit reference is made to the retention of heads are Spencer (1986), who reanalyses Amahl’s data

[1]  John Kingston,et al.  Between the grammar and physics of speech , 1994 .

[2]  D. Steriade Greek prosodies and the nature of syllabification , 1982 .

[3]  Philip James Hamilton,et al.  Phonetic constraints and markedness in the phonotactics of Australian Aboriginal languages , 1996 .

[4]  J. Stemberger,et al.  Handbook of Phonological Development: From the Perspective of Constraint-Based Nonlinear Phonology , 1999 .

[5]  Heather Goad,et al.  Phonetic evidence for phonological structure in syllabification , 2003 .

[6]  C. Stoel-Gammon,et al.  Patterns of early lexical and phonological development , 1984, Journal of Child Language.

[7]  Monik Charette,et al.  Conditions on Phonological Government , 1991 .

[8]  Keren Rice,et al.  On deriving sonority: a structural account of sonority relationships , 1992, Phonology.

[9]  H. Goad Consonant harmony in child language , 1997 .

[10]  Heather Goad,et al.  Consonant Harmony in Child Language: An Optimality-theoretic Account* , 1998 .

[11]  W. Zonneveld,et al.  H. Van der Hulst. Syllable Structure and Stress in Dutch. Dordrecht: Foris. Pp. 260. $25.00. , 1986, Canadian Journal of Linguistics/Revue canadienne de linguistique.

[12]  Junko Ito,et al.  Feature Predictability and Underspecification: Palatal Prosody in Japanese Mimetics , 1989 .

[13]  Junko Ito,et al.  Sympathy theory and German truncations , 1997 .

[14]  John Harris,et al.  Licensing Inheritance: an integrated theory of neutralisation , 1998, Phonology.

[15]  Thomas F. Shannon,et al.  The Syllable in Dutch, with Special Reference to Diminutive Formation , 1988 .

[16]  D. Ohala,et al.  The influence of sonority on children's cluster reductions. , 1999, Journal of Communication Disorders.

[17]  Harry van der Hulst,et al.  Syllable Structure and Stress in Dutch , 1985 .

[18]  P. Fikkert On the acquisition of prosodic structure , 1994 .

[19]  A. Spencer Towards a theory of phonological development , 1986 .

[20]  Rebecca Treiman,et al.  The syllabification of /s/ clusters in English , 1992 .

[21]  Charles Reiss,et al.  Formal and Empirical Arguments concerning Phonological Acquisition , 1998, Linguistic Inquiry.

[22]  Marie-Hélène Côté,et al.  Consonant cluster phonotactics : a perceptual approach , 2000 .

[23]  A. Gnanadesikan Constraints in Phonological Acquisition: Markedness and faithfulness constraints in child phonology , 2004 .

[24]  Julie Levin A metrical theory of syllabicity , 1985 .

[25]  G. Clements Papers in Laboratory Phonology: The role of the sonority cycle in core syllabification , 1990 .

[26]  H. V. Velten The growth of phonemic and lexical patterns in infant language , 1943 .

[27]  Marlys A. Macken,et al.  The child's lexical representation: the ‘puzzle-puddle-pickle’ evidence , 1980, Journal of Linguistics.

[28]  John Alderete Faithfulness to Prosodic Heads , 2001 .

[29]  Heinz J. Giegerich Metrical Phonology and Phonological Structure: German and English , 1985 .

[30]  Jean Lowenstamm,et al.  Constituent structure and government in phonology , 1990, Phonology.

[31]  Richard Wiese,et al.  The Phonology of German , 1996 .

[32]  John Harris,et al.  English Sound Structure , 1994 .

[33]  D. Steriade Phonetics in Phonology: The Case of Laryngeal Neutralization , 1999 .

[34]  Wick R. Miller,et al.  Acoma grammar and texts , 1965 .

[35]  Alan S. Prince,et al.  Faithfulness and reduplicative identity , 1995 .

[36]  J. Gierut Syllable onsets: clusters and adjuncts in acquisition. , 1999, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[37]  R. Kager,et al.  SCHWA, SYLLABLES, AND EXTRAMETRICALITY IN DUTCH , 1986 .

[38]  J. Barlow,et al.  Optimality theory in phonological acquisition. , 1999, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[39]  C. Lleó,et al.  Consonant clusters in child phonology and the directionality of syllable structure assignment , 1996, Journal of Child Language.

[40]  L. Leonard,et al.  Do children pick and choose? an examination of phonological selection and avoidance in early lexical acquisition , 1982, Journal of Child Language.

[41]  GLYNE PIGGOTT,et al.  APOCOPE AND THE LICENSING OF EMPTY-HEADED SYLLABLES , 1991 .

[42]  Steven Pinker,et al.  Language learnability and language development , 1985 .

[43]  W. Zonneveld,et al.  Phonological Acquisition and Dutch Word Prosody , 1996 .

[44]  Yvan Rose,et al.  A Structural Account of Root Node Deletion in Loanword Phonology , 1999, Canadian Journal of Linguistics/Revue canadienne de linguistique.

[45]  Sharon Rose Theoretical issues in comparative Ethio-Semitic phonology and morphology , 2000 .

[46]  Sharon Inkelas,et al.  The consequences of Optimization for Underspecification , 1994 .

[47]  Emmanuel Nikiema Government-licensing and consonant cluster simplification in Quebec French , 1999 .

[48]  Joe Pater Non-uniformity in English secondary stress: the role of ranked and lexically specific constraints , 2000, Phonology.

[49]  D. G. Gilbers,et al.  Compensatory Lengthening and Cluster Reduction in First Language Acquisition : A Comparison of Different Analyses , 1994 .

[50]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  Lectures on Government and Binding , 1981 .

[51]  Richard Wright,et al.  Consonant Clusters and Cue Preservation in Tsou , 1996 .

[52]  P. Smolensky On the comprehension/production dilemma in child language , 1996 .

[53]  Yvan Rose Headedness and Prosodic Licensing in the L1 Acquisition of Phonology , 2000 .

[54]  H. Goad Assimilation Phenomena and Initial Constraint Ranking in Early Grammars , 2001 .

[55]  D. Ingram Phonological Disability in Children , 1976 .

[56]  J. Barlow,et al.  A constraint-based account of syllable onsets. Evidence from developing systems , 1999 .

[57]  Junko Ito,et al.  Prosodic faithfulness and correspondence: Evidence from a Japanese argot , 1996 .

[58]  Theo Vennemann ZUR SILBENSTRUKTUR DER DEUTSCHEN STANDARDSPRACHE , 1982 .

[59]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  The Sound Pattern of English , 1968 .

[60]  Michael Kenstowicz,et al.  Phonology In Generative Grammar , 1994 .

[61]  G. Booij The Phonology of Dutch , 1995 .

[62]  Heinrich-Heine,et al.  The Lack of Onsets in German Child Phonology * , 2000 .

[63]  C. A. Ferguson,et al.  WORDS AND SOUNDS IN EARLY LANGUAGE ACQUISITION , 1975 .

[64]  B. Elan Dresher,et al.  Head–dependent asymmetries in phonology: complexity and visibility , 1998, Phonology.

[65]  J. Kaye On the syllable structure of certain West African languages , 1985 .

[66]  伊藤 順子 Syllable theory in prosodic phonology , 1986 .

[67]  Joe Pater Minimal Violation and Phonological Development , 1997 .

[68]  Jeroen van de Weijer,et al.  Segmental Structure and Complex Segments , 1996 .

[69]  Jessica A. Barlow,et al.  An argument for adjuncts: Evidence from a phonologically disordered system , 1999 .

[70]  T. A. Hall Syllable structure and syllable-related processes in German , 1992 .

[71]  W. Zonneveld,et al.  Schwa, superheavies, stress and syllables in Dutch , 1993 .

[72]  Richard Wiese Silbische und lexikalische Phonologie : Studien zum Chinesischen und Deutschen , 1988 .

[73]  S. B. Chin,et al.  Consonant clusters in disordered speech: constraints and correspondence patterns , 1992, Journal of Child Language.

[74]  R. Jakobson Child Language, Aphasia and Phonological Universals , 1980 .

[75]  Katherine Demuth,et al.  Markedness and the Development of Prosodic Structure , 1995 .