Losing on all fronts: the effects of negative versus positive person-based campaigns on implicit and explicit evaluations of political candidates.

The current research investigated the effects of negative as compared to positive person-based political campaigns on explicit and implicit evaluations of the involved candidates. Participants were presented with two political candidates and statements that one of them ostensibly said during the last political campaign. For half of the participants, the campaign included positive remarks about the source of the statement (positive campaign); for the remaining half, the campaign included negative remarks about the opponent (negative campaign). Afterwards, participants completed measures of explicit and implicit evaluations of both candidates. Results indicate that explicit evaluations of the source, but not the opponent, were less favourable after negative as compared to positive campaigns. In contrast, implicit evaluations were less favourable for both candidates after negative campaigns. The results are discussed in terms of associative and propositional processes, highlighting the importance of associative processes in political decision making.

[1]  Russell H. Fazio,et al.  Attitudes and cognitive consistency: The role of associative and propositional processes , 2008 .

[2]  John J. Skowronski,et al.  Spontaneous trait transference to familiar communicators: is a little knowledge a dangerous thing? , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[3]  N. Roese,et al.  Backlash Effects in Attack Politics1 , 1993 .

[4]  B. Nagengast,et al.  Evaluative conditioning in social psychology: Facts and speculations , 2005, Cognition & emotion.

[5]  Ted Brader,et al.  Striking a Responsive Chord: How Political Ads Motivate and Persuade Voters by Appealing to Emotions , 2005 .

[6]  D. B. Matthews,et al.  The Black-Sheep Effect: How Positive and Negative Advertisements Affect Voters' Perceptions of the Sponsor of the Advertisement1 , 1998 .

[7]  M. Banaji,et al.  Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. , 1995, Psychological review.

[8]  Bertram Gawronski,et al.  The TAR Effect: When the Ones Who Dislike Become the Ones Who Are Disliked , 2008, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[9]  Richard R. Lau,et al.  The Effects of Negative Political Advertisements: A Meta-Analytic Assessment , 1999, American Political Science Review.

[10]  Olesya Govorun,et al.  An inkblot for attitudes: affect misattribution as implicit measurement. , 2005, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[11]  Janine Dermody,et al.  Perceptions of negative political advertising: meaningful or menacing? An empirical study of the 1997 British General Election Campaign , 2000 .

[12]  P. Kenney,et al.  Do Negative Campaigns Mobilize or Suppress Turnout? Clarifying the Relationship between Negativity and Participation , 1999, American Political Science Review.

[13]  Lynda Lee Kaid,et al.  Negative versus Positive Television Advertising in U.S. Presidential Campaigns, 1960–1988 , 1991 .

[14]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  Attitudinal Dissociation: What Does It Mean? , 2009 .

[15]  M. Zanna,et al.  Impact of Negative Advertising on Evaluations of Political Candidates: The 1993 Canadian Federal Election , 1997 .

[16]  F. Strack,et al.  4 Attitudes and Cognitive Consistency * The Role of Associative and Propositional Processes , 2022 .

[17]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  Implicit Attitude Generalization Occurs Immediately; Explicit Attitude Generalization Takes Time , 2008, Psychological science.

[18]  F. Strack,et al.  Reflective and Impulsive Determinants of Social Behavior , 2004, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[19]  Lee Sigelman,et al.  The Effects of Negative Political Campaigns: A Meta-Analytic Reassessment , 2007, The Journal of Politics.

[20]  Charles S. Taber,et al.  Activation of Political Attitudes: A Psychophysiological Examination of the Hot Cognition Hypothesis , 2003 .

[21]  A. Greenwald,et al.  Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[22]  D. Carlston,et al.  Spontaneous trait transference: communicators taken on the qualities they describe in others. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[23]  D. Niven A Field Experiment on the Effects of Negative Campaign Mail on Voter Turnout in a Municipal Election , 2006 .

[24]  Ronald Paul Hill,et al.  An Exploration of Voter Responses to Political Advertisements , 1989 .

[25]  L. L. Kaid,et al.  “Masculine” vs. “feminine” strategies in political ADS: Implications for female candidates , 1987 .

[26]  John J. Skowronski,et al.  Linking versus thinking: evidence for the different associative and attributional bases of spontaneous trait transference and spontaneous trait inference. , 2005, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[27]  Lynda Lee Kaid,et al.  Effects of the Television Spots on Images of Dole and Clinton , 1997 .

[28]  K. Goldstein,et al.  Campaign Advertising and Voter Turnout: New Evidence for a Stimulation Effect , 2002, The Journal of Politics.

[29]  M. Lodge,et al.  The Automaticity of Affect for Political Leaders, Groups, and Issues: An Experimental Test of the Hot Cognition Hypothesis , 2005 .

[30]  G. Bodenhausen,et al.  Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: an integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. , 2006, Psychological bulletin.

[31]  Chingching Chang,et al.  Effects of Gender Schematic Processing on the Reception of Political Commercials for Men and Women Candidates , 1995 .

[32]  Alan G. Sawyer,et al.  The sleeper effect in persuasion: a meta-analytic review. , 2004, Psychological bulletin.

[33]  M. Lodge,et al.  Experiments on the Automaticity of Political Beliefs and Attitudes , 2006 .

[34]  Alessandro Amadori,et al.  Predicting the Vote: Implicit Attitudes as Predictors of the Future Behavior of Decided and Undecided Voters , 2008 .

[35]  R. Fazio,et al.  Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: a bona fide pipeline? , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[36]  Etienne P LeBel,et al.  What Do Implicit Measures Tell Us?: Scrutinizing the Validity of Three Common Assumptions , 2007, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[37]  Bertram Gawronski,et al.  Automatic Mental Associations Predict Future Choices of Undecided Decision-Makers , 2008, Science.

[38]  Paul S. Martin Inside the Black Box of Negative Campaign Effects: Three Reasons Why Negative Campaigns Mobilize , 2004 .

[39]  張卿卿 The impacts of emotion elicited by print political advertising on candidate evaluation , 1998 .

[40]  Nicholas A. Valentino,et al.  Does Attack Advertising Demobilize the Electorate? , 1994, American Political Science Review.

[41]  Bertram Gawronski,et al.  Understanding the Relations Between Different Forms of Racial Prejudice: A Cognitive Consistency Perspective , 2008, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[42]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  THE GO/NO-GO ASSOCIATION TASK , 2001 .

[43]  Luigi Castelli,et al.  The Implicit and Explicit Effects of Negative Political Campaigns: Is the Source Really Blamed? , 2010 .

[44]  S. Iyengar,et al.  Going Negative: How Political Advertisements Shrink and Polarize the Electorate , 1995 .

[45]  Craig Leonard Brians,et al.  Campaign Issue Knowledge and Salience: Comparing Reception from TV Commercials, TV News and Newspapers , 1996 .

[46]  T. L. Budesheim,et al.  Persuasiveness of in-group and out-group political messages : The case of negative political campaigning , 1996 .

[47]  Etienne P. LeBel,et al.  Understanding patterns of attitude change: When implicit measures show change, but explicit measures do not ☆ , 2008 .

[48]  J. de Houwer,et al.  Associative learning of likes and dislikes: a review of 25 years of research on human evaluative conditioning. , 2001, Psychological bulletin.

[49]  D. Westen,et al.  RATS, We Should Have Used Clinton: Subliminal Priming in Political Campaigns , 2008 .

[50]  W. Hofmann,et al.  When and why do implicit measures predict behaviour? Empirical evidence for the moderating role of opportunity, motivation, and process reliance , 2008 .

[51]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  NOSEK AND BANAJI T HE G O / NO-GO ASSOCIAT ION TASK THE GO / NO-GO ASSOCIATION TASK , 2001 .

[52]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  Moderators of the relationship between implicit and explicit evaluation. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[53]  Norbert Schwarz,et al.  Implicit measures of attitudes , 2007 .

[54]  Michael A. Olson,et al.  Implicit measures in social cognition. research: their meaning and use. , 2003, Annual review of psychology.

[55]  John G. Geer,et al.  Remembering Attack Ads: An Experimental Investigation of Radio , 2003 .

[56]  R. Banse,et al.  Implicit bias in impression formation: associations influence the construal of individuating information , 2003 .

[57]  M. Shapiro,et al.  Comparing Positive and Negative Political Advertising on Radio , 1992 .

[58]  Gina M. Garramone,et al.  Appeals and strategies of negative political advertising , 1988 .

[59]  Bertram Gawronski,et al.  Unraveling the Processes Underlying Evaluation: Attitudes from the Perspective of the Ape Model , 2007 .

[60]  Bertram Gawronski,et al.  On the propositional nature of cognitive consistency: Dissonance changes explicit, but not implicit attitudes , 2004 .

[61]  R. Lau,et al.  Effects of Negative Campaigning on Turnout in U.S. Senate Elections, 1988-1998 , 2001, The Journal of Politics.

[62]  C. Judd,et al.  Evidence for racial prejudice at the implicit level and its relationship with questionnaire measures. , 1997, Journal of personality and social psychology.