Post-conflict behaviour of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in the Budongo forest, Uganda

Since de Waal & van Roosmalen (1979) first documented the occurrence of reconciliation between former opponents in captive chimpanzees, the study of the post-conflict behaviour of primates has provided valuable information about some of the details of primate social organisation. The vast majority of these studies have been carried out on captive subjects and it has been assumed that these findings are representative of wild primates. We set out to investigate whether this was true for the Sonso community of wild chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii ) of the Budongo Forest, Uganda, using controlled procedures comparable with those used in captive studies. We found that these chimpanzees were much less likely to reconcile than their captive counterparts. Only one dimension of relationship quality had an effect on the likelihood of reconciliation. Individuals which were highly compatible, in terms of time spent affiliating, reconciled conflicts more often than those with weak relationships. Captive chimpanzees have also been shown to 'console' one another (de Waal & van Roosmalen, 1979; de Waal & Aureli, 1996), where uninvolved bystanders initiate affiliative contacts with victims of aggression. This study did not confirm that consolatory behaviour was characteristic of wild chimpanzee post-conflict behaviour. Nor did these chimpanzees use explicit gestures during post-conflict interactions as they have been shown to do in two out of three captive studies. We conclude that the post-conflict behaviour of chimpanzees is more variable than has previously been thought and is likely to be dependent on the prevailing social environment.

[1]  F. Waal,et al.  Reconciliation and Redirected Affection in Rhesus Monkeys , 1983 .

[2]  Steven J. Schwager,et al.  A comparison of association indices , 1987, Animal Behaviour.

[3]  M. Cords Post-conflict reunions and reconciliation in long-tailed macaques , 1992, Animal Behaviour.

[4]  W. J. Eggeling,et al.  Observations on the Ecology of the Budongo Rain Forest, Uganda , 1947 .

[5]  Filippo Aureli,et al.  Consolation, reconciliation, and a possible cognitive difference between macaques and chimpanzees , 1996 .

[6]  Janet Goodall,et al.  The recent rise to the alpha-rank in a population of free-living chimpanzees. , 1977, Folia primatologica; international journal of primatology.

[7]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Chimpanzees know what conspecifics do and do not see , 2000, Animal Behaviour.

[8]  D. Watts Redirection, consolation and male policing : how targets of aggression interact with bystanders , 2000 .

[9]  G. Schino,et al.  Primate displacement activities as an ethopharmacological model of anxiety. , 1996, Anxiety.

[10]  H. Kummer,et al.  On the value of social relationships to nonhuman primates: A heuristic scheme , 1978 .

[11]  F. Aureli,et al.  Long-tailed macaques avoid conflicts during short-term crowding , 1995 .

[12]  F. Aureli,et al.  Methodological improvements for the study of reconciliation , 1994, Behavioural Processes.

[13]  G. Schino RECONCILIATION IN DOMESTIC GOATS , 1998 .

[14]  Josep Call,et al.  Influence of kinship and spatial density on reconciliation and grooming in rhesus monkeys , 1996, American journal of primatology.

[15]  B. Thierry Clasping Behaviour in Macaca Tonkeana , 1984 .

[16]  C. V. van Schaik,et al.  Functional aspects of reconciliation among captive long‐tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) , 1989, American journal of primatology.

[17]  Shuichi Matsumura,et al.  Postconflict affiliative contacts between former opponents among wild moor macaques (Macaca maurus) , 1996, American journal of primatology.

[18]  F. Waal,et al.  Effects of spatial crowding on social behavior in a chimpanzee colony , 1982 .

[19]  D. L. Castles,et al.  Variation in conciliatory tendency and relationship quality across groups of pigtail macaques , 1996, Animal Behaviour.

[20]  R. Wrangham,et al.  Range use of the forest chimpanzees of Kibale: Implications for the understanding of chimpanzee social organization , 1993, American journal of primatology.

[21]  F. Aureli,et al.  Differential kinship effect on reconciliation in three species of macaques (Macaca fascicularis, M. fuscata, and M. sylvanus). , 1997, Journal of comparative psychology.

[22]  Charlotte K. Hemelrijk,et al.  Reciprocity and interchange of grooming and ‘support’ in captive chimpanzees , 1991, Animal Behaviour.

[23]  T. Rowell A quantitative comparison of the behaviour of a wild and a caged baboon group. , 1967, Animal behaviour.

[24]  R. Seyfarth,et al.  Redirected aggression and reconciliation among vervet monkeys, Cercopithecus aethiops , 1989 .

[25]  Kate Arnold Post-conflict behaviour of spectacled Langurs (Trachypithecus obscurus) , 1997 .

[26]  D. Povinelli,et al.  What young chimpanzees know about seeing. , 2000, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[27]  S. Siegel,et al.  Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences , 2022, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[28]  V. Reynolds,et al.  The effects of selective logging in monodominant tropical forests on biodiversity. Final report , 1997 .

[29]  Andrew J. Plumptre,et al.  Changes following 60 years of selective timber harvesting in the Budongo Forest Reserve, Uganda , 1996 .

[30]  F. D. de Waal,et al.  Dominance style of Japanese macaques compared with rhesus and stumptail macaques , 1995, American journal of primatology.

[31]  R. Byrne,et al.  Machiavellian intelligence : social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans , 1990 .

[32]  N. Newton-Fisher Tactical behaviour and decision making in wild chimpanzees , 1997 .

[33]  Daniel J. Povinelli,et al.  Comprehension of role reversal in chimpanzees: evidence of empathy? , 1992, Animal Behaviour.

[34]  T. Ljungberg,et al.  Post‐Conflict Affiliation in Two Captive Groups of Black‐and‐White Guereza Colobus guereza , 2000 .

[35]  F. Aureli,et al.  Kin-oriented redirection among Japanese macaques: an expression of a revenge system? , 1992, Animal Behaviour.

[36]  Charlotte K. Hemelrijk,et al.  Great Ape Societies: Reciprocation in apes: from complex cognition to self-structuring , 1996 .

[37]  F. D. de Waal,et al.  Toward a comparative socioecology of the genus Macaca: Different dominance styles in rhesus and stumptail monkeys , 1989, American journal of primatology.

[38]  M. Festa‐Bianchet,et al.  Social dominance in adult female mountain goats , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[39]  Robin I. M. Dunbar Primate social systems , 1987 .

[40]  A. Pusey,et al.  The influence of dominance rank on the reproductive success of female chimpanzees. , 1997, Science.

[41]  F. Waal,et al.  Conflict avoidance among rhesus monkeys: coping with short-term crowding , 1993, Animal Behaviour.

[42]  F. Waal,et al.  Rhesus monkey behaviour under diverse population densities: coping with long-term crowding , 1997, Animal Behaviour.

[43]  V. Reynolds Chimpanzees in the Budongo Forest, 1962–1992 , 1992 .

[44]  F. Aureli,et al.  Behavioural Indicators of Anxiety: an Empirical Test in Chimpanzees , 1997 .

[45]  Josep Call,et al.  Reconciliation patterns among stumptailed macaques: a multivariate approach , 1999, Animal Behaviour.

[46]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know? , 2001, Animal Behaviour.

[47]  A. Whiten Machiavellian Intelligence II: The Machiavellian mindreader , 1997 .

[48]  C. Hemelrijk,et al.  Side-Directed Behaviour and Recruitment of Support in Captive Chimpanzees , 1991 .

[49]  P. Judge Dyadic and triadic reconciliation in pigtail macaques (Macaca nemestrina) , 1991, American journal of primatology.

[50]  Charlotte K. Hemelrijk,et al.  ‘Friendship’ for fitness in chimpanzees? , 1999, Animal Behaviour.

[51]  J. Altmann,et al.  Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. , 1974, Behaviour.

[52]  F. Aldrich-Blake Long-Tailed Macaques , 1980 .

[53]  Luca Rosati,et al.  Intragroup Variation in Conciliatory Tendencies in Captive Japanese Macaques , 1998 .