The brain uses adaptive internal models of scene statistics for sensorimotor estimation and planning

Significance To test the hypothesis that the brain uses adaptive models of object statistics to interpret sensory information, we measured the statistical models used by subjects to estimate object speed when asked to hit a moving object. Subjects’ behavior showed perceptual biases to the mean speed within a stimulus set that accurately adapted to changes in the variance of a stimulus set. More significantly, the results show that the way that stimuli on one trial influence following trials adapts appropriately to changes in trial-to-trial correlations in a stimulus set, although subjects’ estimates of correlations retain erroneous positive biases. Because of uncertainty and noise, the brain should use accurate internal models of the statistics of objects in scenes to interpret sensory signals. Moreover, the brain should adapt its internal models to the statistics within local stimulus contexts. Consider the problem of hitting a baseball. The impoverished nature of the visual information available makes it imperative that batters use knowledge of the temporal statistics and history of previous pitches to accurately estimate pitch speed. Using a laboratory analog of hitting a baseball, we tested the hypothesis that the brain uses adaptive internal models of the statistics of object speeds to plan hand movements to intercept moving objects. We fit Bayesian observer models to subjects’ performance to estimate the statistical environments in which subjects’ performance would be ideal and compared the estimated statistics with the true statistics of stimuli in an experiment. A first experiment showed that subjects accurately estimated and used the variance of object speeds in a stimulus set to time hitting behavior but also showed serial biases that are suboptimal for stimuli that were uncorrelated over time. A second experiment showed that the strength of the serial biases depended on the temporal correlations within a stimulus set, even when the biases were estimated from uncorrelated stimulus pairs subsampled from the larger set. Taken together, the results show that subjects adapted their internal models of the variance and covariance of object speeds within a stimulus set to plan interceptive movements but retained a bias to positive correlations.

[1]  B. C. Griffith,et al.  The discrimination of speech sounds within and across phoneme boundaries. , 1957, Journal of experimental psychology.

[2]  H. Helson Adaptation-level theory : an experimental and systematic approach to behavior , 1964 .

[3]  R. Teghtsoonian,et al.  On the exponents in Stevens' law and the constant in Ekman's law. , 1971, Psychological review.

[4]  A. Tversky,et al.  Subjective Probability: A Judgment of Representativeness , 1972 .

[5]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  Sensitivity of smooth eye movement to small differences in target velocity , 1987, Vision Research.

[6]  G. Orban,et al.  Human velocity and direction discrimination measured with random dot patterns , 1988, Vision Research.

[7]  D. Lyon,et al.  Time course of visual extrapolation accuracy. , 1995, Acta psychologica.

[8]  Yaakov Kareev,et al.  Positive bias in the perception of covariation. , 1995 .

[9]  P R DeLucia,et al.  Cognitive motion extrapolation and cognitive clocking in prediction motion task. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  Jack L. Vevea,et al.  Why do categories affect stimulus judgment? , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[11]  E. Brenner,et al.  The effect of expectations on hitting moving targets: influence of the preceding target's speed , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  Edward H. Adelson,et al.  Motion illusions as optimal percepts , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  R C Miall,et al.  System Identification Applied to a Visuomotor Task: Near-Optimal Human Performance in a Noisy Changing Task , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  K. Sailor,et al.  Is memory for stimulus magnitude Bayesian? , 2005, Memory & cognition.

[15]  M. Miyazaki,et al.  Testing Bayesian models of human coincidence timing. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  G R Barnes,et al.  Attention and selection for predictive smooth pursuit eye movements. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[17]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  Noise characteristics and prior expectations in human visual speed perception , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[18]  M. Ernst,et al.  The statistical determinants of adaptation rate in human reaching. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[19]  Ellen Poliakoff,et al.  Typical object velocity influences motion extrapolation , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  Nava Rubin,et al.  Bi-stable depth ordering of superimposed moving gratings. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[21]  Ellen Poliakoff,et al.  The effect of previously viewed velocities on motion extrapolation , 2008, Vision Research.

[22]  R. J. Beers,et al.  Motor Learning Is Optimally Tuned to the Properties of Motor Noise , 2009, Neuron.

[23]  Karl R. Gegenfurtner,et al.  Precision of speed discrimination and smooth pursuit eye movements , 2009, Vision Research.

[24]  Hugo Merchant,et al.  Subsecond timing in primates: comparison of interval production between human subjects and rhesus monkeys. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Learning Priors for Bayesian Computations in the Nervous System , 2010, PloS one.

[26]  Aaron R. Seitz,et al.  Rapidly learned stimulus expectations alter perception of motion. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[27]  C S Green,et al.  Alterations in choice behavior by manipulations of world model , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[28]  Michael N. Shadlen,et al.  Temporal context calibrates interval timing , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[29]  Philip N. Sabes,et al.  How Each Movement Changes the Next: An Experimental and Theoretical Study of Fast Adaptive Priors in Reaching , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  Optimal inference explains the perceptual coherence of visual motion stimuli. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[31]  F. Petzschner,et al.  Iterative Bayesian Estimation as an Explanation for Range and Regression Effects: A Study on Human Path Integration , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  Dominic W. Massaro,et al.  Psychophysical scaling : Judgments of attributes or objects ? , 2015 .

[33]  F. Urban The Central Tendency of Judgment. , 2022 .