Contrast sensitivity during the initiation of smooth pursuit eye movements

Eye movements challenge the perception of a stable world by inducing retinal image displacement. During saccadic eye movements visual stability is accompanied by a remapping of visual receptive fields, a compression of visual space and perceptual suppression. Here we explore whether a similar suppression changes the perception of briefly presented low contrast targets during the initiation of smooth pursuit eye movements. In a 2AFC design we investigated the contrast sensitivity for threshold-level stimuli during the initiation of smooth pursuit and during saccades. Pursuit was elicited by horizontal step-ramp and ramp stimuli. At any time from 200 ms before to 500 ms after pursuit stimulus onset, a blurred 0.3 deg wide horizontal line with low contrast just above detection threshold appeared for 10 ms either 2 deg above or below the pursuit trajectory. Observers had to pursue the moving stimulus and to indicate whether the target line appeared above or below the pursuit trajectory. In contrast to perceptual suppression effects during saccades, no pronounced suppression was found at pursuit onset for step-ramp motion. When pursuit was elicited by a ramp stimulus, pursuit initiation was accompanied by catch-up saccades, which caused saccadic suppression. Additionally, contrast sensitivity was attenuated at the time of pursuit or saccade stimulus onset. This attenuation might be due to an attentional deficit, because the stimulus required the focus of attention during the programming of the following eye movement.

[1]  L. Stark,et al.  The main sequence, a tool for studying human eye movements , 1975 .

[2]  M. Morrone,et al.  Extraretinal Control of Saccadic Suppression , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  C. Blakemore,et al.  The site of saccadic suppression , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[4]  C. Rashbass,et al.  The relationship between saccadic and smooth tracking eye movements , 1961, The Journal of physiology.

[5]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  Shared attentional control of smooth eye movement and perception , 1986, Vision Research.

[6]  T Moore,et al.  Control of eye movements and spatial attention. , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[7]  F. Bremmer,et al.  An fMRI study of optokinetic nystagmus and smooth-pursuit eye movements in humans , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  Kae Nakamura,et al.  Updating of the visual representation in monkey striate and extrastriate cortex during saccades , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[9]  David Burr,et al.  Suppression of the magnocellular pathway during saccades , 1996, Behavioural Brain Research.

[10]  Robert A. Jacobs,et al.  Comparing perceptual learning across tasks: A review , 2002 .

[11]  M. Lappe,et al.  Contrast dependency of saccadic compression and suppression , 2004, Vision Research.

[12]  C. J. Keemink,et al.  Contrast sensitivity for oscillating sine wave gratings during ocular fixation and pursuit , 1988, Vision Research.

[13]  M. Goldberg,et al.  The time course of perisaccadic receptive field shifts in the lateral intraparietal area of the monkey. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  D. Burr,et al.  Selective suppression of the magnocellular visual pathway during saccadic eye movements , 1994, Nature.

[15]  L. Yumin,et al.  CONTRAST SENSITIVITY MEASURED DURING SMOOTH PURSUIT MOVEMENT , 1984 .

[16]  Hermann Aubert,et al.  Die Bewegungsempfindung , 2005, Archiv für die gesamte Physiologie des Menschen und der Tiere.

[17]  R. Krauzlis,et al.  Tracking with the mind’s eye , 1999, Trends in Neurosciences.

[18]  M. Carrasco,et al.  Spatial covert attention increases contrast sensitivity across the CSF: support for signal enhancement , 2000, Vision Research.

[19]  R. Leigh,et al.  The neurology of eye movements , 2006 .

[20]  Paul van Donkelaar,et al.  Spatiotemporal modulation of attention during smooth pursuit eye movements. , 1999 .

[21]  K. Hoffmann,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Saccadic Suppression , 2002, Science.

[22]  D. Burr,et al.  Changes in visual perception at the time of saccades , 2001, Trends in Neurosciences.

[23]  Karl R Gegenfurtner,et al.  Temporal contrast sensitivity during smooth pursuit eye movements. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[24]  S. B. Stevenson,et al.  Dependence of visual suppression on the amplitudes of saccades and blinks , 1986, Vision Research.

[25]  D. Robinson,et al.  Shared neural control of attentional shifts and eye movements , 1996, Nature.

[26]  R. Krauzlis The Control of Voluntary Eye Movements: New Perspectives , 2005, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[27]  A. Tomlinson,et al.  A comparison of saccadic and blink suppression in normal observers , 1997, Vision Research.

[28]  D. M. MACKAY,et al.  Elevation of Visual Threshold by Displacement of Retinal Image , 1970, Nature.

[29]  C. Genovese,et al.  Remapping in human visual cortex. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  A. Wertheim,et al.  An Acceleration Illusion Caused by Underestimation of Stimulus Velocity during Pursuit Eye Movements: Aubert–Fleischl Revisited , 1990, Perception.

[31]  Paul T. Sowden,et al.  Perceptual learning of luminance contrast detection: specific for spatial frequency and retinal location but not orientation , 2002, Vision Research.

[32]  C. Colby,et al.  Spatial updating in area LIP is independent of saccade direction. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  Bart Krekelberg,et al.  Neural Correlates of Saccadic Suppression in Humans , 2004, Current Biology.

[34]  P. Latour Visual threshold during eye movements , 1962 .

[35]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Oculocentric spatial representation in parietal cortex. , 1995, Cerebral cortex.

[36]  U. Ilg Slow eye movements , 1997, Progress in Neurobiology.

[37]  Harry J. Wyatt,et al.  Detecting saccades with jerk , 1998, Vision Research.

[38]  Ernst von Fleischl,et al.  Physiologisch-optische Notizen , 1882 .

[39]  Brian J. Murphy,et al.  Pattern thresholds for moving and stationary gratings during smooth eye movement , 1978, Vision Research.

[40]  L Mitrani,et al.  Dependence of visual suppression on the angular size of voluntary saccadic eye movements. , 1970, Vision research.

[41]  M. Ibbotson,et al.  Enhanced motion sensitivity follows saccadic suppression in the superior temporal sulcus of the macaque cortex. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[42]  J R Duhamel,et al.  The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements. , 1992, Science.

[43]  R. Gellman,et al.  Human smooth pursuit: stimulus-dependent responses. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  S. Lisberger,et al.  Properties of visual inputs that initiate horizontal smooth pursuit eye movements in monkeys , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[45]  R. Angel,et al.  Visual suppression during smooth following and saccadic eye movements. , 1969, Vision research.

[46]  D. Burr,et al.  Selective depression of motion sensitivity during saccades. , 1982, The Journal of physiology.

[47]  B. Dosher,et al.  The role of attention in the programming of saccades , 1995, Vision Research.

[48]  D. Kerzel,et al.  Visual short-term memory during smooth pursuit eye movements. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[49]  Uwe J. Ilg,et al.  Cancellation of self-induced retinal image motion during smooth pursuit eye movements , 2001, Vision Research.

[50]  Richard J Krauzlis,et al.  Shared Response Preparation for Pursuit and Saccadic Eye Movements , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  Katherine M. Armstrong,et al.  Selective gating of visual signals by microstimulation of frontal cortex , 2003, Nature.

[52]  I. Rock,et al.  Inattentional blindness: An overview , 2003 .

[53]  R. Krauzlis Recasting the smooth pursuit eye movement system. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[54]  Ignacio Vallines,et al.  Saccadic Suppression of Retinotopically Localized Blood Oxygen Level-Dependent Responses in Human Primary Visual Area V1 , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[55]  E. Holst,et al.  Das Reafferenzprinzip , 2004, Naturwissenschaften.

[56]  H. Deubel,et al.  Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism , 1996, Vision Research.

[57]  Vision Research , 1961, Nature.

[58]  E Herman,et al.  Position Constancy during Pursuit Eye Movement: An Investigation of the Filehne Illusion , 1973, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[59]  F. Bremmer,et al.  Visual receptive field modulation in the lateral intraparietal area during attentive fixation and free gaze. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[60]  Robert M. McPeek,et al.  Saccades require focal attention and are facilitated by a short-term memory system , 1999, Vision Research.

[61]  M. Chun,et al.  The dark side of visual attention , 2002, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[62]  K. Uchikawa,et al.  Saccadic suppression of achromatic and chromatic responses measured by increment-threshold spectral sensitivity. , 1995, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[63]  D. W. Scott On optimal and data based histograms , 1979 .